Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane Australia

Case study: using social cognitive theory and social support coping theory to improve breastfeeding duration rates: MumBubConnect

Russell-Bennett, Rebekah , Gallegos, Danielle , Previte, Josephine , & Hamilton, Robyn (2014) Correction to: Case study: using social cognitive theory and social support coping theory to improve breastfeeding duration rates: MumBubConnect. In Aleti, T , Binney, W , Parker, L , Nguyen, D , & Brennan, L (Eds.) Social marketing and behaviour change: Models, theory and applications. Edward Elgar Publishing, United Kingdom, pp. 66-74.

View at publisher

Description

In this case study, social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986, 2004) and social support as a way of coping (Vitaliano et al., 1985) have been selected to overcome barriers created by low levels of self-confidence and perceived lack of support in the context of breastfeeding. Thus, the application of the two theories in this case study is designed to improve the mothers’ self-efficacy and reinforce the behaviour of breastfeeding. The World Health Organization (WHO) recommends exclusive breastfeeding for the first six months of life (WHO, 2001); however, in Australia (as with many developed countries), breastfeeding duration declines rapidly after three months. A total of 47 per cent of infants are fully breastfed to three months, reducing to 21 per cent being predominantly breastfed to five months (Australian Institute of Health and Welfare [AIHW], 2011a). The national significance of breastfeeding is noted with the release of the National Breastfeeding Strategy in late 2009, which aimed to improve the health of infants, young children and mothers by protecting, promoting, supporting and monitoring breastfeeding (National Health and Medical Research Council [NHMRC], 2013). It is critical that Australia addresses the poor continuation of breastfeeding to protect the next generation of Australians against acute and chronic diseases. A social marketing programme was therefore developed that aimed to test the effect of a technology-based intervention on breastfeeding duration. The intervention was conducted in Australia with participants from every state.

Impact and interest:

Citation counts are sourced monthly from Scopus and Web of Science® citation databases.

These databases contain citations from different subsets of available publications and different time periods and thus the citation count from each is usually different. Some works are not in either database and no count is displayed. Scopus includes citations from articles published in 1996 onwards, and Web of Science® generally from 1980 onwards.

Citations counts from the Google Scholar™ indexing service can be viewed at the linked Google Scholar™ search.

  • Notify us of incorrect data
  • How to use citation counts
  • More information
105530
Chapter in Book, Report or Conference volume (Chapter)
Russell-Bennett, Rebekah
Gallegos, Danielle

Export: EndNote | Dublin Core | BibTeX

Repository Staff Only: item control page

-

  • Browse research
  • TEQSA Provider ID: PRV12079 (Australian University)
  • CRICOS No. 00213J
  • ABN 83 791 724 622
  • Accessibility
  • Right to Information

Social Cognitive Theory: How We Learn From the Behavior of Others

Thomas Barwick/Getty Images 

  • Archaeology

case study social cognitive theory

  • Ph.D., Psychology, Fielding Graduate University
  • M.A., Psychology, Fielding Graduate University
  • B.A., Film Studies, Cornell University

Social cognitive theory is a learning theory developed by the renowned Stanford psychology professor Albert Bandura. The theory provides a framework for understanding how people actively shape and are shaped by their environment. In particular, the theory details the processes of observational learning and modeling, and the influence of self-efficacy on the production of behavior.

Key Takeaways: Social Cognitive Theory

  • Social cognitive theory was developed by Stanford psychologist Albert Bandura.
  • The theory views people as active agents who influence and are influenced by their environment.
  • A major component of the theory is observational learning: the process of learning desirable and undesirable behaviors by observing others, and then reproducing learned behaviors to maximize rewards.
  • Individuals' beliefs in their own self-efficacy influence whether or not they will reproduce an observed behavior.

Origins: The Bobo Doll Experiments

In the 1960s, Bandura, along with his colleagues, initiated a series of well-known studies on observational learning called the Bobo Doll experiments. In the first of these experiments, preschool children were exposed to an aggressive or nonaggressive adult model to see if they would imitate the model’s behavior. The gender of the model was also varied, with some children observing same-sex models and some observing opposite-sex models.

In the aggressive condition, the model was verbally and physically aggressive towards an inflated Bobo doll in the presence of the child. After exposure to the model, the child was taken to another room to play with a selection of highly attractive toys. To frustrate participants, the child’s play was stopped after about two minutes. At that point, the child was taken to a third room filled with different toys, including a Bobo doll, where they were allowed to play for the next 20 minutes.

The researchers found that the children in the aggressive condition were much more likely to display verbal and physical aggression, including aggression towards the Bobo doll and other forms of aggression. In addition, boys were more likely to be aggressive than girls, especially if they had been exposed to an aggressive male model.

A subsequent experiment utilized a similar protocol, but in this case, the aggressive models were not just seen in real life. There was also a second group that observed a film of the aggressive model as well as a third group that observed a film of an aggressive cartoon character. Again, the gender of the model was varied, and the children were subjected to mild frustration before they were brought to the experimental room to play. As in the previous experiment, the children in the three aggressive conditions exhibited more aggressive behavior than those in the control group and boys in the aggressive condition exhibited more aggression than girls.

These studies served as the basis for ideas about observational learning and modeling in real life and through the media. In particular, it spurred a debate that continues today over the ways media models can negatively influence children. 

In 1977, Bandura introduced Social Learning Theory, which further refined his ideas on observational learning and modeling. Then in 1986, Bandura renamed his theory Social Cognitive Theory to put greater emphasis on the cognitive components of observational learning and the way behavior, cognition, and the environment interact to shape people.

Observational Learning

Observational learning is a major component of social cognitive theory. Bandura’s ideas about learning stood in contrast to those of behaviorists like B.F. Skinner . According to Skinner, learning could only be achieved by taking individual action. However, Bandura claimed that observational learning, through which people observe and imitate models they encounter in their environment, enables people to acquire information much more quickly.

Observational learning occurs through a sequence of four processes :

  • Attentional processes account for the information that is selected for observation in the environment. People might select to observe real-life models or models they encounter via media.
  • Retention processes involve remembering the observed information so it can be successfully recalled and reconstructed later.
  • Production processes reconstruct the memories of the observations so that what was learned can be applied in appropriate situations. In many cases, this does not mean the observer will replicate the observed action exactly, but that they will modify the behavior to produce a variation that fits the context.
  • Motivational processes determine whether or not an observed behavior is performed, based on whether that behavior was observed to result in desired or adverse outcomes for the model. If an observed behavior is rewarded, the observer will be more motivated to reproduce it later. However, if a behavior is punished in some way, the observer would be less motivated to reproduce it. Thus, social cognitive theory cautions that people do not perform every behavior they learn through modeling.

Self-Efficacy

In addition to the information that models can convey during observational learning, models can also increase or decrease the observer’s belief in their self-efficacy to enact observed behaviors and bring about desired outcomes from those behaviors. When people see others like them succeed, they also believe they can be capable of succeeding. Thus, models are a source of motivation and inspiration.

Perceptions of self-efficacy influence people’s choices and beliefs in themselves, including the goals they choose to pursue and the effort they put into them, how long they are willing to persevere in the face of obstacles and setbacks, and the outcomes they expect. Thus, self-efficacy influences one’s motivations to perform various actions and one's belief in their ability to do so.

Such beliefs can impact personal growth and change. For example, research has shown that enhancing self-efficacy beliefs is more likely to result in the improvement of health habits than the use of fear-based communication. Belief in one’s self-efficacy can be the difference between whether or not an individual even considers making positive changes in their life.

Modeling Media

The prosocial potential of media models has been demonstrated through serial dramas that were produced for developing communities on issues such as literacy, family planning, and the status of women. These dramas have been successful in bringing about positive social change while demonstrating the relevance and applicability of social cognitive theory to media.

For example, a television show in India was produced to raise women’s status and promote smaller families by embedding these ideas in the show. The show championed gender equality by including characters that positively modeled women’s equality. In addition, other characters modeled subservient women’s roles and some transitioned between subservience and equality. The show was popular, and despite its melodramatic narrative, viewers understood the messages it modeled. These viewers learned that women should have equal rights, the freedom to choose how they live their lives, and the ability to limit the size of their families. In this example and others, the tenets of social cognitive theory have been utilized to make a positive impact through fictional media models.

  • Bandura, Albert. “Social cognitive theory for personal and social change by enabling media.” Entertainment-education and social change: History, research, and practice , edited by Arvind Singhal, Michael J. Cody, Everett M. Rogers, and Miguel Sabido, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2004, pp. 75-96.
  • Bandura, Albert. “Social Cognitive Theory of Mass Communication. Media Psychology , vol. 3, no. 3, 2001, pp. 265-299, https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0303_03
  • Bandura, Albert. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory . Prentice Hall, 1986.
  • Bandura, Albert, Dorothea Ross, and Sheila A. Ross. “Transmission of Aggression Through Imitation of Aggressive Models.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, vol. 63, no. 3, 1961, pp. 575-582, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0045925
  • Bandura, Albert, Dorothea Ross, and Sheila A. Ross. “Imitation of Film-Mediated Aggressive Models.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, vol. 66, no. 1, 1961, pp. 3-11, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0048687
  • Crain, William. Theories of Development: Concepts and Applications . 5th ed., Pearson Prentice Hall, 2005.
  • What Is Self-Concept in Psychology?
  • Social Constructionism Definition and Examples
  • What Is Uses and Gratifications Theory? Definition and Examples
  • Maslow's Hierarchy of Needs Explained
  • Psychodynamic Theory: Approaches and Proponents
  • Freud: Id, Ego, and Superego Explained
  • Information Processing Theory: Definition and Examples
  • Understanding Sexual Orientation From a Psychological Perspective
  • What Is a Schema in Psychology? Definition and Examples
  • Cognitive Dissonance Theory: Definition and Examples
  • Gender Schema Theory Explained
  • How Psychology Defines and Explains Deviant Behavior
  • Understanding Social Identity Theory and Its Impact on Behavior
  • What Is Socioemotional Selectivity Theory?
  • What Is the Schachter-Singer Theory of Emotion?
  • What Is Cognitive Bias? Definition and Examples

Albert Bandura’s Social Cognitive Theory

Charlotte Nickerson

Research Assistant at Harvard University

Undergraduate at Harvard University

Charlotte Nickerson is a student at Harvard University obsessed with the intersection of mental health, productivity, and design.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Saul McLeod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

Key Takeaways

  • Social cognitive theory emphasizes the learning that occurs within a social context. In this view, people are active agents who can both influence and are influenced by their environment.
  • The theory was founded most prominently by Albert Bandura, who is also known for his work on observational learning, self-efficacy, and reciprocal determinism.
  • One assumption of social learning is that we learn new behaviors by observing the behavior of others and the consequences of their behavior.
  • If the behavior is rewarded (positive or negative reinforcement), we are likely to imitate it; however, if the behavior is punished, imitation is less likely. For example, in Bandura and Walters’ experiment,  the children imitated more the aggressive behavior of the model who was praised for being aggressive to the Bobo doll.
  • Social cognitive theory has been used to explain a wide range of human behavior, ranging from positive to negative social behaviors such as aggression, substance abuse, and mental health problems.

social cognitive theory 1

How We Learn From the Behavior of Others

Social cognitive theory views people as active agents who can both influence and are influenced by their environment.

The theory is an extension of social learning that includes the effects of cognitive processes — such as conceptions, judgment, and motivation — on an individual’s behavior and on the environment that influences them.

Rather than passively absorbing knowledge from environmental inputs, social cognitive theory argues that people actively influence their learning by interpreting the outcomes of their actions, which, in turn, affects their environments and personal factors, informing and altering subsequent behavior (Schunk, 2012).

By including thought processes in human psychology, social cognitive theory is able to avoid the assumption made by radical behaviorism that all human behavior is learned through trial and error. Instead, Bandura highlights the role of observational learning and imitation in human behavior.

Numerous psychologists, such as Julian Rotter and the American personality psychologist Walter Mischel, have proposed different social-cognitive perspectives.

Albert Bandura (1989) introduced the most prominent perspective on social cognitive theory.

Bandura’s perspective has been applied to a wide range of topics, such as personality development and functioning, the understanding and treatment of psychological disorders, organizational training programs, education, health promotion strategies, advertising and marketing, and more.

The central tenet of Bandura’s social-cognitive theory is that people seek to develop a sense of agency and exert control over the important events in their lives.

This sense of agency and control is affected by factors such as self-efficacy, outcome expectations, goals, and self-evaluation (Schunk, 2012).

Origins: The Bobo Doll Experiments

Social cognitive theory can trace its origins to Bandura and his colleagues, in particular, a series of well-known studies on observational learning known as the Bobo Doll experiments .

bobo doll experiment

In these experiments, researchers exposed young, preschool-aged children to videos of an adult acting violently toward a large, inflatable doll.

This aggressive behavior included verbal insults and physical violence, such as slapping and punching. At the end of the video, the children either witnessed the aggressor being rewarded, or punished or received no consequences for his behavior (Schunk, 2012).

After being exposed to this model, the children were placed in a room where they were given the same inflatable Bobo doll.

The researchers found that those who had watched the model either received positive reinforcement or no consequences for attacking the doll were more likely to show aggressive behavior toward the doll (Schunk, 2012).

This experiment was notable for being one that introduced the concept of observational learning to humans.

Bandura’s ideas about observational learning were in stark contrast to those of previous behaviorists, such as B.F. Skinner.

According to Skinner (1950), learning can only be achieved through individual action.

However, Bandura claimed that people and animals can also learn by watching and imitating the models they encounter in their environment, enabling them to acquire information more quickly.

Observational Learning

Bandura agreed with the behaviorists that behavior is learned through experience. However, he proposed a different mechanism than conditioning.

He argued that we learn through observation and imitation of others’ behavior.

This theory focuses not only on the behavior itself but also on the mental processes involved in learning, so it is not a pure behaviorist theory.

Social Learning Theory Bandura four stages mediation process in social learning theory attention retention motor reproduction motivation in diagram flat style.

Stages of the Social Learning Theory (SLT)

Not all observed behaviors are learned effectively. There are several factors involving both the model and the observer that determine whether or not a behavior is learned. These include attention, retention, motor reproduction, and motivation (Bandura & Walters, 1963).

The individual needs to pay attention to the behavior and its consequences and form a mental representation of the behavior. Some of the things that influence attention involve characteristics of the model.

This means that the model must be salient or noticeable. If the model is attractive, prestigious, or appears to be particularly competent, you will pay more attention. And if the model seems more like yourself, you pay more attention.

Storing the observed behavior in LTM where it can stay for a long period of time. Imitation is not always immediate. This process is often mediated by symbols. Symbols are “anything that stands for something else” (Bandura, 1998).

They can be words, pictures, or even gestures. For symbols to be effective, they must be related to the behavior being learned and must be understood by the observer.

Motor Reproduction

The individual must be able (have the ability and skills) to physically reproduce the observed behavior. This means that the behavior must be within their capability. If it is not, they will not be able to learn it (Bandura, 1998).

The observer must be motivated to perform the behavior. This motivation can come from a variety of sources, such as a desire to achieve a goal or avoid punishment.

Bandura (1977) proposed that motivation has three main components: expectancy, value, and affective reaction. Firstly, expectancy refers to the belief that one can successfully perform the behavior. Secondly, value refers to the importance of the goal that the behavior is meant to achieve.

The last of these, Affective reaction, refers to the emotions associated with the behavior.

If behavior is associated with positive emotions, it is more likely to be learned than a behavior associated with negative emotions. Reinforcement and punishment each play an important role in motivation.

Individuals must expect to receive the same positive reinforcement (vicarious reinforcement) for imitating the observed behavior that they have seen the model receiving.

Imitation is more likely to occur if the model (the person who performs the behavior) is positively reinforced. This is called vicarious reinforcement.

Imitation is also more likely if we identify with the model. We see them as sharing some characteristics with us, i.e., similar age, gender, and social status, as we identify with them.

Features of Social Cognitive Theory

The goal of social cognitive theory is to explain how people regulate their behavior through control and reinforcement in order to achieve goal-directed behavior that can be maintained over time.

Bandura, in his original formulation of the related social learning theory, included five constructs, adding self-efficacy to his final social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986).

Reciprocal Determinism

Reciprocal determinism is the central concept of social cognitive theory and refers to the dynamic and reciprocal interaction of people — individuals with a set of learned experiences — the environment, external social context, and behavior — the response to stimuli to achieve goals.

Its main tenet is that people seek to develop a sense of agency and exert control over the important events in their lives.

This sense of agency and control is affected by factors such as self-efficacy, outcome expectations, goals, and self-evaluation (Bandura, 1989).

To illustrate the concept of reciprocal determinism, Consider A student who believes they have the ability to succeed on an exam (self-efficacy) is more likely to put forth the necessary effort to study (behavior).

If they do not believe they can pass the exam, they are less likely to study. As a result, their beliefs about their abilities (self-efficacy) will be affirmed or disconfirmed by their actual performance on the exam (outcome).

This, in turn, will affect future beliefs and behavior. If the student passes the exam, they are likely to believe they can do well on future exams and put forth the effort to study.

If they fail, they may doubt their abilities (Bandura, 1989).

Behavioral Capability

Behavioral capability, meanwhile, refers to a person’s ability to perform a behavior by means of using their own knowledge and skills.

That is to say, in order to carry out any behavior, a person must know what to do and how to do it. People learn from the consequences of their behavior, further affecting the environment in which they live (Bandura, 1989).

Reinforcements

Reinforcements refer to the internal or external responses to a person’s behavior that affect the likelihood of continuing or discontinuing the behavior.

These reinforcements can be self-initiated or in one’s environment either positive or negative. Positive reinforcements increase the likelihood of a behavior being repeated, while negative reinforcers decrease the likelihood of a behavior being repeated.

Reinforcements can also be either direct or indirect. Direct reinforcements are an immediate consequence of a behavior that affects its likelihood, such as getting a paycheck for working (positive reinforcement).

Indirect reinforcements are not immediate consequences of behavior but may affect its likelihood in the future, such as studying hard in school to get into a good college (positive reinforcement) (Bandura, 1989).

Expectations

Expectations, meanwhile, refer to the anticipated consequences that a person has of their behavior.

Outcome expectations, for example, could relate to the consequences that someone foresees an action having on their health.

As people anticipate the consequences of their actions before engaging in a behavior, these expectations can influence whether or not someone completes the behavior successfully (Bandura, 1989).

Expectations largely come from someone’s previous experience. Nonetheless, expectancies also focus on the value that is placed on the outcome, something that is subjective from individual to individual.

For example, a student who may not be motivated to achieve high grades may place a lower value on taking the steps necessary to achieve them than someone who strives to be a high performer.

Self-Efficacy

Self-efficacy refers to the level of a person’s confidence in their ability to successfully perform a behavior.

Self-efficacy is influenced by a person’s own capabilities as well as other individual and environmental factors.

These factors are called barriers and facilitators (Bandura, 1989). Self-efficacy is often said to be task-specific, meaning that people can feel confident in their ability to perform one task but not another.

For example, a student may feel confident in their ability to do well on an exam but not feel as confident in their ability to make friends.

This is because self-efficacy is based on past experience and beliefs. If a student has never made friends before, they are less likely to believe that they will do so in the future.

Modeling Media and Social Cognitive Theory

Learning would be both laborious and hazardous in a world that relied exclusively on direct experience.

Social modeling provides a way for people to observe the successes and failures of others with little or no risk.

This modeling can take place on a massive scale. Modeling media is defined as “any type of mass communication—television, movies, magazines, music, etc.—that serves as a model for observing and imitating behavior” (Bandura, 1998).

In other words, it is a means by which people can learn new behaviors. Modeling media is often used in the fashion and taste industries to influence the behavior of consumers.

This is because modeling provides a reference point for observers to imitate. When done effectively, modeling can prompt individuals to adopt certain behaviors that they may not have otherwise engaged in.

Additionally, modeling media can provide reinforcement for desired behaviors.

For example, if someone sees a model wearing a certain type of clothing and receives compliments for doing so themselves, they may be more likely to purchase clothing like that of the model.

Observational Learning Examples

There are numerous examples of observational learning in everyday life for people of all ages.

Nonetheless, observational learning is especially prevalent in the socialization of children. For example:

  • A newer employee avoids being late to work after seeing a colleague be fired for being late.
  • A new store customer learns the process of lining up and checking out by watching other customers.
  • A traveler to a foreign country learning how to buy a ticket for a train and enter the gates by witnessing others do the same.
  • A customer in a clothing store learns the procedure for trying on clothes by watching others.
  • A person in a coffee shop learns where to find cream and sugar by watching other coffee drinkers locate the area.
  •  A new car salesperson learning how to approach potential customers by watching others.
  •  Someone moving to a new climate and learning how to properly remove snow from his car and driveway by seeing his neighbors do the same.
  •  A tenant learning to pay rent on time as a result of seeing a neighbor evicted for late payment.
  •  An inexperienced salesperson becomes successful at a sales meeting or in giving a presentation after observing the behaviors and statements of other salespeople.
  •  A viewer watches an online video to learn how to contour and shape their eyebrows and then goes to the store to do so themselves.
  •  Drivers slow down after seeing that another driver has been pulled over by a police officer.
  •  A bank teller watches their more efficient colleague in order to learn a more efficient way of counting money.
  •  A shy party guest watching someone more popular talk to different people in the crowd, later allowing them to do the same thing.
  • Adult children behave in the same way that their parents did when they were young.
  • A lost student navigating a school campus after seeing others do it on their own.

Social Learning vs. Social Cognitive Theory

Social learning theory and Social Cognitive Theory are both theories of learning that place an emphasis on the role of observational learning.

However, there are several key differences between the two theories. Social learning theory focuses on the idea of reinforcement, while Social Cognitive Theory emphasizes the role of cognitive processes.

Additionally, social learning theory posits that all behavior is learned through observation, while Social Cognitive Theory allows for the possibility of learning through other means, such as direct experience.

Finally, social learning theory focuses on individualistic learning, while Social Cognitive Theory takes a more holistic view, acknowledging the importance of environmental factors.

Though they are similar in many ways, the differences between social learning theory and Social Cognitive Theory are important to understand. These theories provide different frameworks for understanding how learning takes place.

As such, they have different implications in all facets of their applications (Reed et al., 2010).

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory . Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84 (2), 191.

 Bandura, A. (1986). Fearful expectations and avoidant actions as coeffects of perceived self-inefficacy.

Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American psychologist, 44 (9), 1175.

Bandura, A. (1998). Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychology and health, 13 (4), 623-649.

Bandura, A. (2003). Social cognitive theory for personal and social change by enabling media. In Entertainment-education and social change (pp. 97-118). Routledge.

Bandura, A. Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through the imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology , 63, 575-582.

LaMort, W. (2019). The Social Cognitive Theory. Boston University.

Reed, M. S., Evely, A. C., Cundill, G., Fazey, I., Glass, J., Laing, A., … & Stringer, L. C. (2010). What is social learning?. Ecology and society, 15 (4).

Schunk, D. H. (2012). Social cognitive theory .

Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary?. Psychological Review, 57 (4), 193.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection

Logo of pheelsevier

Social cognition theories and behavior change in COVID-19: A conceptual review

Martin s. hagger.

a Department of Psychological Sciences, University of California, Merced, 5200 N. Lake Rd., Merced, CA, 95343, USA

b Health Sciences Research Institute, University of California, Merced, 5200 N. Lake Rd., Merced, CA, 95343, USA

c Faculty of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Jyväskylä, PO Box 35, FI-40014, Jyväskylä, Finland

d School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Mt. Gravatt Campus, 176 Messines Ridge Rd, Mt. Gravatt, QLD, 4122, Australia

Kyra Hamilton

e Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, G40 Griffith Health Centre, Level 8.86, Gold Coast Campus, QLD, 4222, Australia

The COVID-19 pandemic has had unprecedented health, economic, and social consequences worldwide. Although contact reductions and wearing face coverings have reduced infection rates, and vaccines have reduced illness severity, emergence of new variants of the coronavirus that causes COVID-19, and the shift from pandemic to endemic patterns of infection, highlights the importance of ongoing preventive behavior adherence to manage future outbreaks. Research applying social cognition theories may assist in explaining variance in these behaviors and inform the development of efficacious behavior change interventions to promote adherence. In the present article, we summarize research applying these theories to identify modifiable determinants of COVID-19 preventive behaviors and the mechanisms involved, and their utility in informing interventions. We identify limitations of these applications (e.g., overreliance on correlational data, lack of long-term behavioral follow-up), and suggest how they can be addressed. We demonstrate the virtue of augmenting theories with additional constructs (e.g., moral norms, anticipated regret) and processes (e.g., multiple action phases, automatic processes) to provide comprehensive, parsimonious behavioral explanations. We also outline how the theories contribute to testing mechanisms of action of behavioral interventions. Finally, we recommend future studies applying these theories to inform and test interventions to promote COVID-19 preventive behavior adherence.

1. Introduction

Since the COVID-19 outbreak was declared a global pandemic in March 2020 ( WHO, 2020 ), the disease has contributed substantially to excess deaths globally ( CDC, 2021b ), resulted in damaging economic outcomes (e.g., unemployment and redundancy, increased numbers of people falling below the poverty line, business bankruptcy and closure; Jackson et al., 2021 ), placed immense strain on healthcare services (e.g., reduced capacity to provide elective services and long-term care, increased stress on staff, elevated health risks for frontline workers; Bartsch et al., 2020 ), and led to deleterious social consequences (e.g., increased incidence of mental health difficulties including depression, suicidality, loneliness, and isolation; Holmes et al., 2020 ). Lockdown and other legislated mitigation procedures together with the rapid development and administration of vaccines have made important contributions to curbing infection rates, particularly severe cases, as well reducing economic, healthcare, and social strains, such that people worldwide have begun to envision a post-pandemic world ( Kashte, Gulbake, El-Amin, & Gupta, 2021 ). However, the emergent threat of new highly-virulent variants of the coronavirus that causes COVID-19 infections, such as the delta and omicron variants, highlights the reality that the pandemic is far from over, and that there will likely be need to maintain mitigation and management procedures for some time in the future to bring infection rates under control ( Wu et al., 2021 ). Furthermore, even if mitigation procedures lead to COVID-19 infection rates to fall below pandemic levels, complete eradication of the virus is unlikely ( Phillips, 2021 ). Rather, it is more likely to become endemic, similar to other viral infections such as influenza and the common cold. This means that people worldwide may need to be prepared to manage localized and seasonal outbreaks in future ( Phillips, 2021 ).

Throughout the pandemic, behavior change has been central to the effective management of COVID-19 infection rates ( Michie, 2020 ; West, Michie, Rubin, & Amlôt, 2020 ). Populations worldwide have become eminently familiar with a raft of COVID-19 preventive behaviors including physical distancing, wearing face coverings, avoiding large gatherings particularly indoors, hand sanitization, and adherence to transmission prevention guidelines (e.g., self-isolation, quarantine). Further key preventive behaviors have also become cornerstone in the mitigation of coronavirus transmission and minimizing infection severity, including uptake of COVID-19 vaccines and booster shots ( Bar-On et al., 2021 ; CDC, 2021a ; JVCI, 2021 ) and rapid antigen testing ( Crozier, Rajan, Buchan, & McKee, 2021 ). However, despite extensive messaging and advocacy of these COVID-19 preventive behaviors by government agencies and public health services, engagement in these behaviors has been highly variable, and often falls short of the levels necessary to bring infection rates under control, particularly when they are at their peak ( Mathieu et al., 2021 ). This has particularly been the case for vaccine uptake, where adherence rates vary considerably but often fall below those necessary to attain the widespread immunity to halt subsequent waves of infection ( Mathieu et al., 2021 ). Further, increased documentation of ‘breakthrough’ infections among the vaccinated population ( Bergwerk et al., 2021 ), and the need to offer protection to those vulnerable to serious cases of COVID-19 (e.g., vaccine allergic individuals, individuals with underlying conditions, and the immunosuppressed), means that other COVID-19 preventive behaviors such as physical distancing and wearing face coverings are still necessary even among those who have been vaccinated.

Given the ongoing pandemic and its future management necessitates continuity of interventions promoting uptake and maintenance of COVID-19 preventive behaviors, the need for effective messaging and health communication has come to the fore. Governmental and public health agencies have turned to behavioral science to identify strategies that will promote attention to, assimilation of, and responsivity to messaging interventions around COVID-preventive behaviors ( Albarracín & Jung, 2021 ; Bonell et al., 2020 ; Michie, Rubin, & Amlôt, 2020 ; Michie et al., 2020 ; West et al., 2020 ). Importantly, drawing from a substantive body of prior research on behavior change, behavioral scientists have highlighted the imperative for messaging interventions to be based on a fundamental understanding of human behavior ( Hagger, Cameron, et al., 2020 ; Michie et al., 2018 ). Central to this understanding is the need to base interventions on behavioral theory ( Hagger, Moyers, McAnally, & McKinley, 2020 ; Prestwich, Webb, & Conner, 2015 ), a contention predicated on evidence that theory-based behavioral interventions demonstrate greater efficacy and efficiency ( Hagger & Weed, 2019 ; McEwan et al., 2019 ; Prestwich et al., 2014 ). Theory provides a basis for providing a priori predictions on how interventions work, that is, the extent to which the intervention is expected to change behavior and the mechanisms involved, and provides a means to falsify predictions relating to efficacy and mechanism against observation ( Rothman, Klein, & Sheeran, 2020 ; Sheeran, Klein, & Rothman, 2017 ). In addition, basing interventions on theory enables specification of the inter- and intra-personal, socio-structural, and contextual conditions that are expected to enhance or mitigate their efficacy ( Hagger, Gucciardi, & Chatzisarantis, 2017 ; Rothman & Sheeran, 2020 ; Trafimow, 2012 ).

Social cognition theories feature prominently among theories that have been applied to predict behavior and behavior change, and have made important contributions to identifying health behavior determinants and the mechanisms involved ( Conner and Norman, 2015a , Conner and Norman, 2015b ; Fishbein et al., 2001 ). Social cognition theories adopt a reasoned action approach, assuming that engagement in a given target behavior is a function of individuals' beliefs or judgements with respect to their future performance of the behavior ( Fishbein & Ajzen, 2010 ). Importantly, the beliefs are considered potentially modifiable through information-provision strategies presented in behavioral interventions (e.g., messages highlighting the utility of, available support for, and personal capacity to perform the behavior; Ajzen & Schmidt, 2020 ; Hamilton & Johnson, 2020 ). The theories also specify the processes by which these strategies ‘work’ to change behavior, known as mechanisms of action ( Hagger, Cameron, et al., 2020 ; Hagger, Moyers, et al., 2020 ; Sheeran et al., 2017 ). A large corpus of research applying social cognition approaches in health contexts has supported their predictions and demonstrated that their constructs account for non-trivial variance in behavior (e.g., Carpenter, 2010 ; McEachan, Conner, Taylor, & Lawton, 2011 ; Milne, Sheeran, & Orbell, 2000 ). In the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, social cognition theories have featured prominently in studies predicting COVID-19 preventive behaviors (for a review see Albarracín & Jung, 2021 ). These studies have been successful in accounting for unique variance in behavior, and have assisted in identifying relevant processes (e.g., Bogg & Milad, 2020 ; Chu & Liu, 2021 ; Hagger, Smith, Keech, Moyers, & Hamilton, 2020 ; Hamilton, Smith, Keech, Moyers, & Hagger, 2020 ; Jang, Kim, & Kwon, 2021 ; Norman, Wilding, & Conner, 2020 ; Tong, He, Wu, Dang, & Chen, 2021 ). In addition, their application has also demonstrated utility in informing the development of behavioral interventions in this context – interventions based on these theories have demonstrated efficacy in changing behavior (e.g., Ahn, Hu, & Vega, 2021 ; Keller et al., 2021 ; Okuhara, Okada, & Kiuchi, 2020 ; Smith, Hagger, Keech, Moyers, & Hamilton, 2021 ).

Despite these successful applications, numerous limitations and knowledge gaps remain in the application of these theories in this context such as a preponderance of evidence based on cross-sectional and correlational data; a lack of behavioral data and, particularly, data on long-term behavioral prediction and the prediction of change in behavior; a lack of experimental and intervention research; and few tests of the theory-based mechanisms by which the interventions change behavior. In this article, we aim to summarize the contribution of research applying social cognition theories to predict and change behavior in the context of COVID-19. Specifically, we outline the value of social cognition theories in identifying the determinants and possible targets for intervention in health behavior, focusing on COVID-19 preventive behaviors (e.g., maintaining physical distancing, wearing face coverings, avoiding group gatherings; getting vaccinated, participating in rapid testing); provide a critique of the application of these theories including identifying some of their prominent limitations and boundary conditions (e.g., prediction of intentions rather than behavior, confinement to short term prediction, lack of specificity in beliefs, overuse of correlational designs), and their implications for COVID-19 prevention; summarize some emergent innovations in the research in this context (e.g., integration of other constructs, testing of mechanisms of action) that seek to address these limitations and advance knowledge on behavioral determinants and behavior change in this context; and identify evidence gaps (e.g., the transition from pandemic to endemic illness management, accounting for disparities and inequality in health behavior, dealing with traits and individual differences) and provide some suggested future advancements that may contribute to addressing these gaps.

2. Social cognition theories and COVID-19 preventive behaviors

2.1. social cognition theories: basic assumptions and supporting evidence.

Social cognition theories have their origins in reasoned action approaches that assume individuals’ decisions on whether or not to engage in a given target behavior are based on their processing of the available information relating to the behavior and their evaluation of it ( Ajzen, 1991 ; Conner, 2015 ; Fishbein & Ajzen, 2010 ; Fishbein et al., 2001 ). Specifically, the theories predict that individuals form belief-based judgements or expectations that represent or summarize information about the behavior and make their decisions on action accordingly. Social cognitive theory ( Bandura, 1986 ), the health belief model ( Rosenstock, 1974 ), protection motivation theory ( Rogers, 1975 ), and the theories of reasoned action ( Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980 ) and planned behavior ( Ajzen, 1991 ) are leading examples of social cognition theories, and they have been widely applied.

The belief-based behavioral determinants specified in social cognition theories are summarized in sets of constructs. Common to many of these theories are constructs that represent beliefs regarding the value or utility of the behavior in producing outcomes (e.g., outcome evaluations in social cognitive theory, perceived benefits in the health belief model, response efficacy in protection motivation theory, attitudes in the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior); risk perceptions (e.g., threat appraisals in the health belief model and protection motivation theory); capacity beliefs (e.g., self-efficacy beliefs in social cognitive theory and protection motivation theory, perceived behavioral control in the theory of planned behavior), and social norms (e.g., subjective norms in the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior). A number of theories also specify dispositions to act (e.g., intention in the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior, protection motivation in protection motivation theory), which reflect a readiness to act and willingness to invest effort to pursue the behavior in future, often operationalized as a mediator of effects of belief-based constructs on behavior, a mechanistic prediction ( Ajzen, 1991 ; Fishbein & Ajzen, 2010 ; Rogers, 1975 ; Fishbein et al., 2001 , McMillan and Conner, 2007 ). For example, a prototypical social cognition theory, the theory of planned behavior, predicts that effects of attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control with respect to future performance of the target behavior on behavior are mediated by intentions ( Ajzen, 1991 ).

The basic premises of social cognition theories in the context of health behaviors has been supported in multiple predictive studies (e.g., Carpenter, 2010 ; McEachan et al., 2011 ; Milne et al., 2000 ). Studies typically tap the belief-based constructs from the theories using validated psychometric instruments in which individuals from a target population are prompted to estimate their beliefs with respect to performing a target behavior at some specified time in the future. Valid tests of the theories necessitate subsequent collection of behavior measures within the specified timeframe, and evaluate the extent to which the belief-based constructs predict the behavior using a prospective design. Studies adopting such designs have provided consistent support for the theories in explaining variance in behavior in multiple health behaviors, populations, and contexts. For example, meta-analytic syntheses of these studies demonstrate small-to-medium sized effects of theory constructs on action dispositions and behavior, and have also supported key mechanistic predictions, such as the mediation of the effects of belief-based constructs on behavior by dispositions to act (e.g., Albarracín, Johnson, Fishbein, & Muellerleile, 2001 ; Hamilton, van Dongen, & Hagger, 2020 ; McEachan et al., 2011 ). This meta-analytic research has also been extended to account for other important predictions and auxiliary assumptions of these theories such as the capacity of the theory to account for unique variance in behavior when controlling for past behavior (e.g., Chatzisarantis, Hagger, Smith, & Phoenix, 2004 ; Hagger, Polet, & Lintunen, 2018 ), a test of theory sufficiency , and also for moderator effects among the constructs themselves, such as the moderation of the intention-behavior relationship by perceived behavioral control ( Hagger, Cheung, Ajzen, & Hamilton, 2022 ) or the properties of attitudes ( Cooke & Sheeran, 2004 ) in the theory of planned behavior. Taken together, research has provided basic evidence in support of theory hypotheses with regard to the prediction of behavior and some key proposed mechanisms within the theories.

2.2. Applying social cognition theories in COVID-19

The demonstrated efficacy of social cognition theories in accounting for behavior provides impetus for their application to predict COVID-19 preventive behaviors. Specifically, leading social cognition theories have been adopted to predict intention toward, and actual participation in, general COVID-19 preventive behaviors (e.g., Clark, Davila, Regis, & Kraus, 2020 ; Norman et al., 2020 ; Peterson, Helweg-Larsen, & DiMuccio, 2021 ; Rabin & Dutra, 2021 ), or specific preventive behaviors such as social or physical distancing ( Adiyoso & Wilopo, 2021 ; Das, Abdul Kader Jilani, Uddin, Uddin, & Ghosh, 2021 ; Gibson, Magnan, Kramer, & Bryan, 2021 ; Yu, Lau, & Lau, 2021 ), wearing face coverings (e.g., Barile et al., 2020 ; Irfan et al., 2021 ), hand hygiene (e.g., Derksen, Keller, & Lippke, 2020 ; Luszczynska et al., 2021 ), and COVID-19 testing adherence (e.g., McElfish, Purvis, James, Willis, & Andersen, 2021 ; Vandrevala, Montague, Terry, & Fielder, 2022 ). General trends from this research suggest that beliefs about utility such as attitudes and response efficacy (e.g., Clark et al., 2020 ; Rabin & Dutra, 2021 ; Yu et al., 2021 ), normative beliefs such as subjective and descriptive norms (e.g., Das et al., 2021 ; Gibson et al., 2021 ; Peterson et al., 2021 ), and beliefs about capacity such as self-efficacy and perceived behavioral control (e.g., Adiyoso & Wilopo, 2021 ; Das et al., 2021 ; Norman et al., 2020 ) account for unique variance in intentions or behavior in these behavioral contexts. Beliefs about threat or risk from COVID-19, such as risk perceptions or perceived severity and susceptibility, have also been shown to have unique effects on intentions and behavior for these behaviors (e.g., Betsch et al., 2021 ; Vandrevala et al., 2022 ), but effect sizes tend to be modest by comparison, or even non-significant, when included as predictors in parallel other theory constructs ( Adiyoso & Wilopo, 2021 ; Derksen et al., 2020 ; Hamilton, Smith, et al., 2020 ; Rabin & Dutra, 2021 ). This is consistent with research indicating that beliefs reflecting behavioral engagement tend to account for substantially more variance in intention and behavior than beliefs about risk from the conditions they are purported to prevent ( Hagger & Orbell, 2021 ) – likely a consequence of the close correspondence between the measures of the beliefs and the behavior, but also because decisions to engage in these preventive behaviors are seldom focused solely on risk reduction.

The theories have also been applied to predict COVID-19 vaccine intentions (e.g., Chu & Liu, 2021 ; Matute, Palau-Saumell, Meyer, Derqui, & Jiménez-Asenjo, 2021 ; Sherman et al., 2021 ; Shiloh, Peleg, & Nudelman, 2021 ). Similar patterns of effects for key social cognition constructs on vaccine intentions have been observed as those found for other COVID-19 preventive behaviors. In a marked deviation, however, trends across these studies suggest that beliefs about the risks of the vaccines themselves and concerns about general vaccine administration such as injections, rather than risks related to COVID-19, are key correlates of vaccine intentions ( Chu & Liu, 2021 ; Hamilton & Hagger, 2022 ; Matute et al., 2021 ; Sherman et al., 2021 ). However, to date, no research has provided a formal comparison of the relative effects of risk perceptions related to COVID-19 and risk perceptions relating to the vaccine itself on COVID-19 vaccination intentions. Futhermore, there a dearth of research on the social cognition correlates of actual vaccine uptake ( Shiloh et al., 2021 ). Taken together, application of social cognition theories has provided initial evidence of the belief-based correlates of COVID-19 preventive behaviors and contribute to an initial evidence base of potentially modifiable constructs that may be targeted in interventions.

2.3. Social cognition theories in COVID-19: limitations and solutions

Studies applying social cognition theories have provided evidence of the constructs associated with COVID-19 preventive behaviors and the mechanisms involved. However, this growing but relatively new research literature has a number of prominent limitations that restrict the extent to which inferences can be drawn from their findings and the degree to which they can be generalized broadly. The limitations include a focus on COVID-19 preventive behavioral intentions rather than behavior itself, with few studies adopting long-term behavioral follow up; a focus on direct or global measures of social cognition constructs, which neglects measures of specific sets of beliefs relevant to COVID-19 preventive behaviors; an over-reliance on cross-sectional, correlational design studies which limit inference of directionality, causality, and change in constructs, particularly behavior, over time; and an exclusive focus on the individual which eschews effects of broader socio-structural factors and environmental context on behavior. In this section we outline the ramifications of these limitations and outline current and future research that may address these limitations.

2.3.1. A focus on intention and short-term prediction

Many studies applying social cognition theories in the context of COVID-19 preventive behaviors have focused on the prediction of intention, with relatively few providing follow-up measures of behavior (e.g., Clark et al., 2020 ; Das et al., 2021 ; Derksen et al., 2020 ). This is particularly the case for vaccine behavior, likely due to the challenges of collecting behavioral data and the relatively short time vaccines have been made available (for an exception, see Shiloh et al., 2021 ). A sole focus on intention is problematic because although intentions are an important theoretical antecedent of behavior, and are often closely associated with behavior in research on social cognition theories (e.g., Carpenter, 2010 ; McEachan et al., 2011 ; Milne et al., 2000 ), the relationship is far from perfect with modest effect sizes observed across studies ( Sheeran & Webb, 2016 ). This intention-behavior ‘gap’ indicates that, for many individuals, intentions may be a necessary but not sufficient condition for behavioral enactment. Including a behavioral follow up in research applying social cognition constructs to predict COVID-19 preventive behaviors is, therefore, important as it not only permits measurement of the variance in behavior accounted for by the theory constructs, but also allows tests of the extent to which social cognition constructs of the theory are mediated by intention and the extent of intention-behavior gap.

In addition, few studies applying social cognition theories have provided long-term follow-up of behavior for any COVID-19 preventive behavior. To date, no study in this context has predicted behavior beyond a few months. This is a substantive evidence gap considering the importance of identifying the determinants of sustained participation in preventive behaviors to minimizing infection rates in the long-term. As a consequence, there is an urgent need for studies that predict behavior over time, and test the extent to which social cognition theories are able to account for sustained engagement in COVID-19 behaviors. Such research should consider adoption of multiple measures of behavior at follow-up and over time periods that extend to a year or more rather than a few weeks. Such predictive studies would have important implications for the potential sustainability of messaging interventions that target change in these beliefs.

2.3.2. Specific beliefs

A further limitation of research applying social cognition theories to COVID-19 behaviors is that they have tended not to account for specific beliefs relating for COVID-19. For example, few studies have investigated specific sets of behavioral, normative, and control beliefs purported to underpin social cognition constructs such as attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control in the theory of planned behavior ( Ajzen, 1991 ), or examined specific outcome expectancies or self-efficacy in the face of specific barriers in social cognitive theory ( Bandura, 1986 ). Similarly, there is relatively little of research focusing on self-efficacy beliefs in the face of specific barriers or facilitating factors to the targeted COVID-19 preventive behavior, or specific expected outcomes with respect to performing the behavior, versions of the self-efficacy and outcome expectancy constructs identified as most potent in accounting for variance in future behavior in the original conceptualization of social cognitive theory ( Bandura, 1986 ). This is problematic given that strategies to change behavior applying the theory necessitates forming persuasive communications that target specific beliefs with respect to the target behavior ( Ajzen & Schmidt, 2020 ; Fishbein & Ajzen, 2010 ; Hamilton & Johnson, 2020 ).

Resolution lies in performing the necessary belief elicitation research to develop expectancy-value indirect measures of constructs in the case of the theory of planned behavior ( Ajzen, 1991 ), or identification of salient outcomes and barriers or facilitating factors in the case of social cognitive theory (see DuCharme & Brawley, 1995 ), and include them as unique predictors of COVID-19 preventive behaviors in predictive studies. Research aimed at identifying specific beliefs in research on COVID-19 preventive behaviors is being conducted, including formal elicitation of beliefs based on social cognition theory guidelines (e.g., Varol et al., 2021 ). In addition, researchers have included additional measures in predictive studies that encompass some specific beliefs. For example, recognition of the moral imperative for engaging in COVID-19 behaviors to protect others from infection, particularly those vulnerable to serious infection such as the elderly and immunocompromised, researchers have included moral norms as an additional predictor alongside other social cognition constructs in predictive studies. While subjective norms reflect perceived social pressure from significant others to perform a COVID-19 preventive behavior, moral norms reflect beliefs that performing the behavior is the socially responsible course of action. Results indicate an important role for this construct as an additional predictor of physical distancing intentions and behavior (e.g., Hagger, Smith, et al., 2020 ; Hagger, Smith, Keech, Moyers, & Hamilton, 2021 ) and general COVID-19 preventive behaviors ( Kojan, Burbach, Ziefle, & Calero Valdez, 2021 ), intention to avoid COVID-19 ( Raza, Ali, & Hussain, 2021 ), and COVID-19 vaccination intentions ( Matute et al., 2021 ) and behavior ( Shiloh et al., 2021 ). These studies have progressed knowledge on the social cognition correlates of COVID-19 preventive behaviors, and we look to future research that further elicits specific beliefs concerning COVID-19 prevention and examines their effects on preventive behaviors.

2.3.3. Study design and inferences

A further limitation is an overreliance on studies adopting cross-sectional and correlational designs to test the predictive validity of these theories in the context of COVID-19 preventive behaviors. Such designs are limited because they do not model change in the theory constructs and in behavior, and preclude inference of causality ( Weinstein, 2007 ). While proposed models specifying the proposed effects of a given social cognition theory may fit well with cross-sectional data measuring theory constructs, equally plausible models that specify a different pattern of effects may fit the data equally well, even if such models may be contraindicated theoretically. In addition, such tests do not rule out that the estimated relations among the constructs on a model could be accounted for by other, unmeasured variables (e.g., other social cognition constructs such as moral norms and anticipated regret, implicit attitudes and motives, dispositional and individual difference constructs). Including a follow-up measure of behavior so as to model past behavior effects on future behavior alongside theory constructs may provide a test of theory sufficiency ( Ajzen, 1991 ; Chatzisarantis et al., 2004 ), but is not informative of these extraneous factors because past behavior is not a psychological construct ( Hagger et al., 2018 ; Ouellette & Wood, 1998 ). As a consequence, predictive studies of this kind provide limited evidence as means to test theoretical predictions, and additional evidence is required to permit more elaborate inferences.

The adoption of cross-lagged panel designs – longitudinal studies in which all constructs and outcomes are measured simultaneously across multiple time points – yield data that permit more elaborate inferences (e.g., Gollob & Reichardt, 1987 ; Liska, Felson, Chamlin, & Baccaglini, 1984 ). Such data allow researchers to control for a certain type of change in constructs over time, called covariance stability, and the extent to which relations among constructs at any given time points vary over time, known as stationarity. These models also enable examination of reciprocal effects among theory constructs, which permits inference of whether constructs predict outcomes, or whether the relationship occurs in the opposite direction, or in both directions over time, for example the effects of behavior on attitudes in addition to the effects of attitudes on behavior ( Albarracín, 2021 ). Research in the context of COVID-19 preventive has demonstrated consistency of effects on social cognition constructs over time (e.g., Hagger et al., 2021 ). However more research is needed, which also needs to take the important step of controlling for effects of localized restrictions, which may affect individuals' beliefs. For example, individuals’ norms or outcome expectancies may vary depending on whether or not mitigation behaviors like wearing face coverings is required – they might feel confident and secure in wearing masks when it is mandated to do so, but might be embarrassed or afraid to do so when restrictions have been dropped. In addition, researchers should also heed calls to adopt optimal approaches to analyzing panel designs ( Usami, 2021 ). In sum, longitudinal panel designs have advantages over cross-sectional or prospective designs by permitting inference on the direction of predicted effects in a theory, and the extent to which it accounts for temporal change in behavior or other outcomes over time.

However, panel designs still do not permit inference of causal effects. An effective means to evaluate causality is to adopt experimental or randomized controlled intervention designs in which the effect of change in a social cognition theory construct as a result of a manipulation of intervention strategy (e.g., provision of information on the benefits or advantages of physical distancing using a persuasive communication to target attitude change) on change in a target behavioral outcome is evaluated ( Imai, Keele, Tingley, & Yamamoto, 2011 ). In such designs, groups of individuals from the target population are randomized to receive the intervention, while groups of individuals randomized in a comparison or control group do not receive the intervention. Intervention effects are evaluated through observed differences in the behavior measured post-intervention across the two groups while simultaneously accounting for pre-intervention variation in behavior across the two groups. Such designs better allow for causal inferences, assuming that randomization was effective and the intervention strategy or method activated change in the targeted theory construct.

There are relatively few studies that have adopted experimental and intervention research to test predictions of social cognition theories in the context of COVID-19 predictive behaviors, which is consistent with research applying social cognition theories more broadly, a trend likely attributable to the greater financial and time cost of research adopting these designs ( Hagger, Cameron, et al., 2020 ). However, there are examples of studies demonstrating that intervention strategies targeting social cognition constructs (e.g., persuasive communication, information provision) have changed intentions toward, and actual participation in, hand hygiene behaviors and staying at home during lockdown (e.g., Capasso, Caso, & Conner, 2021 ; Okuhara et al., 2020 ; Smith et al., 2021 ). These studies provide preliminary evidence that persuasive communications targeting key constructs in social cognition theories lead to changes in preventive behaviors, and consistent with results of intervention studies observed for other health behaviors ( Ajzen & Schmidt, 2020 ; Norman et al., 2018 ; Sheeran et al., 2016 ). However, the literature applying these designs is under developed, with few studies systematically demonstrating concomitant change in social cognition theory constructs and behavior, and or utilizing such designs to verify theoretical predictions, highlighting the need for further research in the area.

2.3.4. An exclusive focus on the individual

A further critique leveled at social cognition theories concerns the almost exclusive focus on individuals' beliefs and how they impact decision making and subsequent behavior. By contrast, relatively little consideration is given to group influences, and the socio-structural and socio-environmental constructs that may also serve to line-up individuals' behavior. This is an important omission considering that individuals’ behavior is not merely a function of behavioral beliefs, but also responses to salient others and group influences (e.g., the pervasiveness of normative influences), and the social environment and context in which the behavior is performed. This is highly pertinent in the context of COVID-19 given that many preventive behaviors are dependent on extraneous factors, such as the presence of lockdown and mask-wearing mandates, availability of face coverings, or layout and available space for physical distancing in indoor areas.

Although there have been attempts to account for the effects of these extraneous factors in tests of social cognition theories by specifying and measuring constructs that reflect individuals' belief-based responses to their social and physical environment, such as norms, these still reflect individuals' beliefs rather than specific group processes that may alter behavioral responses or the extent to which the context places actual constraints on behavior, both of which may not be sufficiently reflected in individuals’ beliefs ( Albarracín, 2021 ). This is in contrast to other theoretical perspectives, such as ecological models of health behavior, which explicitly outline how behavior is a function of factors that operate at multiple levels including the individual, social, and environmental levels ( Sallis, Owen, & Fisher, 2015 ; Salmon, Hesketh, Arundell, Downing, & Biddle, 2020 ). While such models have been widely applied in multiple health behavior contexts, there have been few applications in the context of COVID-19 preventive behaviors with considerable scope for their adoption in future research in this context (e.g., Latkin et al., 2021 ). In addition, there have been attempts to integrate constructs and predictions from social cognition theories with the broader factors derived from ecological models to provide more comprehensive explanations of health behavior and the process involved (e.g., Rhodes, Saelens, & Sauvage-Mar, 2018 ). We return to integrated approaches that incorporate socio-structural variables within social cognition theories and highlight their value in the context of COVID-19 prevention later in the article.

3. Integrated theories: developing comprehensive but parsimonious models

There is general acknowledgment within the behavioral science community that social cognition theories have boundary conditions that delimit the extent to which their inferences apply. As a consequence, theorists and researchers have sought to augment or further develop the theories to expand their range of prediction and increase the comprehensiveness of the behavior explanations they offer ( Hagger, 2009 ; Montaño & Kasprzyk, 2015 ). Foremost among these efforts has been the development of integrated theories that draw constructs and processes from multiple theories and models of human behavior and aim to address boundary conditions of unitary theories. Integrated theories offer two important advances on existing theory: They provide a means to highlight and eliminate redundancy in the constructs and processes used across theories toward the goal of identifying a core set of constructs that have optimal distinctiveness conceptually, and demonstrable discriminant and predictive validity empirically; and they enable the introduction of additional constructs and processes that assist in addressing the boundary conditions that delimit the explanations offered by existing theories ( Hagger, 2009 ; Hagger & Hamilton, 2020 ). Integrated theories, therefore, provide the opportunity to expand the range of falsifiable predictions of existing social cognition theories while retaining optimal parsimony, two key features of a ‘good’ or ‘strong’ theory ( Davis, Campbell, Hildon, Hobbs, & Michie, 2015 ).

3.1. Theory integration: a rationale

A primary goal of theory integration is to promote theory parsimony by identifying a core set of constructs that are sufficiently unique, conceptually and empirically, that form the basis of theory predictions. This has value to those interested in changing behavior by assisting in identifying a core set of psychological constructs that optimally capture the mental processes central to decision making ( McMillan & Conner, 2007 ), and can be activated or changed through intervention ( Avishai, Brewer, Mendel, & Sheeran, 2021 ). However, considerable redundancy has been observed in the definition, conceptualization, operationalization, and measurement of the vast array of social cognition constructs across theories. This is not a new phenomenon, and it has been recognized as a perennial problem in psychology, often referred to as a ‘jangle’ fallacy: multiple constructs with similar content but differing labels ( Block, 1995 ; Hagger, 2014 ; Kelley, 1927 ). Theorists wary of this fallacy have sought to develop means to address this redundancy by developing schemes to analyze the content of constructs across theories in order to identify commonalities and redundancy. In the context of social cognition theories applied in health contexts, researchers have developed similar schemes and have identified core constructs (e.g., McMillan & Conner, 2007 ; Protogerou, Johnson, & Hagger, 2018 ). For example, McMillan and Conner (2007) identified five core constructs across an analysis of multiple social cognition theories: dispositions to act (e.g., protection motivation, intentions), attitudes (e.g., outcome expectancies, affective attitudes), norms (e.g., subjective norms, social support), self-perceptions (e.g., self-esteem), and control (e.g., self-efficacy, perceived behavioral control). These methods are exemplary of an important process that is necessary when integrating theories to minimize redundancy in the constructs adopted and ensure that the constructs represented are likely unique and, therefore, are adequate in capturing the constructs likely to account for variance in behavior.

The major contribution of theory integration to advancing knowledge on individuals' behavior, however, is the introduction of additional processes that address the boundary conditions of extant theories ( Hagger, 2009 ; Hagger & Hamilton, 2020 ; Jacobs, Hagger, Streukens, De Bourdeaudhuij, & Claes, 2011 ). The integrated theories aim to provide more comprehensive behavioral explanations in terms of the mechanisms involved or account for more variance in behavior, and may also pave the way for theory-based interventions that have greater efficacy and efficiency in changing behavior or greater scope in changing behavior across contexts or populations. Of course, development of these integrated approaches needs to be tested against observation in studies adopting appropriate designs to test the predictions of the ‘new’ theory ( Hagger, Gucciardi, & Chatzisarantis, 2017 ). Next, we outline two prominent examples of theory integration using social cognition theories applied to health behavior contexts: integration of dual-phase approaches and planning, and integration of dual-process models to account for non-conscious, automatic processes. We illustrate how these examples have yielded more comprehensive theories and better behavioral explanation. We also demonstrate how these integrations have demonstrated utility advancing the prediction of engagement COVID-19 preventive behaviors.

3.2. Augmenting theories to account for action phases

Integrated theories have been instrumental in addressing the intention-behavior ‘gap’ observed in social cognition theories. The modest association between intention and behavior across social cognition theories applied in health contexts suggests individuals do not necessarily readily act on their intentions ( Orbell & Sheeran, 1998 ; Sheeran & Webb, 2016 ). Researchers have addressed this problem by integrating processes from other theories on self-regulation ( Leventhal, Meyer, & Nerenz, 1980 ), and, particularly, dual-phase models of action, such as the model of action phases ( Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987 ), into traditional social cognition theories like the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior ( Orbell, Hodgkins, & Sheeran, 1997 ). Dual-phase models propose different phases of action, and, in particular, distinguish between an intentional or motivational phase in which intentions to perform a behavior to attain an outcome are formed, and an implemental or volitional phase in which intentions are augmented with specific action plans in order to enact the behavior. It is predicted that the extent to which individuals form plans, particularly plans that link the behavior with salient environmental cues that line up the behavior, enhances intention enactment and strengthens the intention-behavior relationship. Some integrated theories, such as the health action process approach (HAPA; Schwarzer, 2008 ), include multiple phases by design and encompass different types of planning as a conduit between intentions and behavior (for comprehensive descriptions of the HAPA see Schwarzer & Hamilton, 2020 ; Zhang, Zhang, Schwarzer, & Hagger, 2019 ).

Tests of social cognition theories that integrate predictions from dual-phase models have demonstrated that planning constructs account for significant variance in intentions (e.g., Zhang et al., 2019 ), and moderate the intention-behavior association upward (e.g., de Bruijn, Rhodes, & van Osch, 2012 ). In addition, intervention and experimental studies have shown that individuals prompted to form action plans are more likely to follow-through on their intentions (e.g., Armitage, 2004 ; Hagger et al., 2012 ; Orbell et al., 1997 ). Research illustrates that such planning interventions work by promoting greater recall of the intended action ( Orbell et al., 1997 ), and more efficient or ‘automatic’ behavioral enactment ( Martiny-Huenger, Martiny, Parks-Stamm, Pfeiffer, & Gollwitzer, 2017 ) – key mechanisms by which plan formation leads to behavior change. Broad support for these planning interventions has been demonstrated in research syntheses (e.g., Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006 ), illustrating the applied value of integrating a dual-phase approach within social cognition theories in the context of behavior change.

Dual-phase approaches have demonstrable applicability in the context of COVID-19 preventive behaviors ( Harvey, Armstrong, Callaway, Gumport, & Gasperetti, 2021 ). Specifically, research adopting the HAPA has demonstrated associations between intention and planning constructs in studies targeting general COVID-19 preventive behaviors ( Lin et al., 2020 ), and physical distancing ( Beeckman et al., 2020 ; Hamilton, Smith, et al., 2020 ), hand washing ( Lao, Li, Zhao, Gou, & Zhou, 2021 ; Luszczynska et al., 2021 ), and wearing face coverings ( Lao et al., 2021 ) behaviors. Furthermore, direct effects of planning constructs on behavior were found in some of the studies ( Beeckman et al., 2020 ; Lao et al., 2021 ; Lin et al., 2020 ; Luszczynska et al., 2021 ), and that they mediated intention effects on behavior ( Beeckman et al., 2020 ; Lin et al., 2020 ; Luszczynska et al., 2021 ), implicating planning in the process by which intentions are enacted. A specific form of planning, known as implementation intentions or “if-then” plans ( Gollwitzer, 1999 ), has also been proposed as moderator the intention-behavior relationship, suggesting that individuals who plan may be more likely to follow through on their intentions. Studies have indicated that compliance with interventions using if-then plans leads to greater adherence to physical distancing guidelines ( Ahn et al., 2021 ; Bieleke, Martarelli, & Wolff, 2021 ).

These findings illustrate that integration of planning from dual-phase models in social cognition theories broadens their capacity to explain intention-behavior relations and inform interventions to promote COVID-19 preventive behaviors. However, currently available research applying these integrated approaches is relatively sparse and confined to only a few COVID-19 preventive behaviors. In addition, there are very few studies adopting experimental and randomized-controlled designs examining effects of planning strategies on COVID-19 preventive behaviors. There are also a number of outstanding questions that need to be addressed, such as the specific mechanism by which planning leads to behavioral enactment – a mediating process in which planning forms part of the decision-making process, as specified in the HAPA ( Schwarzer, 2008 ), or a moderating process in which planning promotes intention enactment, an explanation consistent with the model of action phases ( Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987 ). There is also little research examinng some of the potential mediators of planning on these behaviors such as recall of the intended behavior and greater accessibility of the behavior when the cue or codnition stated in the plan is presented. We therefore call for studies that test planning effects in a broader range of COVID-19 preventive behaviors, adopt intervention or experimental designs, and mediation and moderation effects.

3.3. Augmenting theories to account for automatic processes

A fundamental assumption of social cognition theories is that actions are a function of reasoned, rational decision making based on available information ( Conner and Norman, 2015a , Conner and Norman, 2015b ; Fishbein et al., 2001 ). An alternative, but not incongruous perspective, is offered by theories of implicit cognition, which outline how stored knowledge structures that link behaviors, contexts, and evaluations lead to behavioral initiation and enactment beyond an individual's awareness ( Hagger, 2016 ; Sheeran, Gollwitzer, & Bargh, 2013 ). These implicit or automatic processes tend to control many everyday behaviors for which elaborate, reasoned decision making is both unnecessary and inefficient, and particularly mundane behaviors with which individuals have copious prior experience ( Gardner, Lally, & Wardle, 2012 ; Wood, Quinn, & Kashy, 2002 ). According to these approaches, individuals learn over time to associate behavioral responses with concomitantly-experienced information such as cues or initiating events or evaluations in memory in organized knowledge structures or schema. Subsequent presentation of a triggering event or evaluation linked to the behavioral response leads to rapid, efficient activation of the behavior. Accordingly, dual-process theories of social cognition have been proposed aimed at providing more comprehensive explanations of behavior and to evaluate the extent to which target behaviors are determined by reasoned and automatic processes, and the conditions that might determine when each process predominates ( Strack & Deutsch, 2004 ; Wood, Labrecque, Lin, & Rünger, 2014 ).

In predictive tests of dual process theories, reasoned processes are represented through the effects of constructs traditionally specified in social cognition theories such as attitudes, norms, risk perceptions, and self-efficacy (e.g., Bandura, 1986 ; Fishbein & Ajzen, 2010 ). In contrast, automatic processes tend to be inferred from constructs representing different types of implicit cognition such as implicit attitudes and motives and measures of habit, (e.g., Hagger, Trost, Keech, Chan, & Hamilton, 2017 ; Hamilton, Gibbs, Keech, & Hagger, 2020 ), or affective desires or behavioral prepotency that reflect impulse-related tendencies, internal drive states, and cue salience (e.g., Gibbons, Gerrard, Blanton, & Russell, 1998 ; Hall & Fong, 2007 ). Incorporating these types of construct in predictive studies of social cognition theories enables researchers to estimate the relative contribution of each to explaining variance in a target behavior. For example, given that implicit cognition is assumed to bypass the reasoned processes that lead to behavior, effects of constructs representing implicit processes are expected to relate to behavior directly, unmediated by intentions. Studies adopting this integrated approach have demonstrated unique effects of implicit attitudes and motives on behavior independent of intentions and other social cognition constructs (e.g., Hamilton, Gibbs, et al., 2020 ; Keatley, Clarke, & Hagger, 2012 ). A similar pattern of effects has been found for measures of habit, a specific form of automaticity that is cue- or context- dependent but unrelated to goal pursuit ( Brown, Hagger, & Hamilton, 2020 ; Hamilton, Kirkpatrick, Rebar, & Hagger, 2017 ; Verplanken & Orbell, 2003 ). In addition, there is research that has illustrated that certain types of behavior, such as those that are less complex, have high propensity to be formed as habits, or demand less deliberative or reasoned consideration, are more likely to be impacted by implicit cognition and habit ( Verplanken, 2006 ). In addition, effects of traditional social cognition constructs have smaller effects on behavior when individuals report higher levels of implicit attitudes or habit. For example, habits and implicit attitudes have been shown to moderate the intention-behavior relationship ( Divine, Berry, Rodgers, & Hall, 2021 ; Gardner, Lally, & Rebar, 2020 ). Taken together, predictive studies applying integrated approaches that incorporate constructs that represent automatic processes have provided useful information on the contribution of non-conscious processes that lead to behavior and the moderating conditions.

Predictive studies examining dual process effects are expected to provide important insight into potentially efficacious intervention strategies. In behavioral contexts where implicit cognition or habit account for non-trivial variance in an undesired behavior, interventionists can select strategies that block or counter cues and initiating events, or promote skills that assist in overriding or managing the behavioral response ( Duckworth, Gendler, & Gross, 2016 ; Gardner, Rebar, & Lally, 2020 ). These include strategies such as environmental or context restructuring, which aim to limit or dampen the salience of the cues (e.g., altering the layout of a grocery store so as to reduce capacity or density of checkout queues or lines), or cue identification, monitoring, and management that aim to provide individuals with an awareness of the conditions that lead to the unwanted behavior (e.g., signage or notices illustrating how viruses spread when an unmasked individual coughs, floor markers showing recommended social distance, provision of hand sanitizer at points of entry to venues), capacity to identify when the cues occur (e.g., facilitating mental imagery of a typical trip to a grocery store and the frequency of contact with people on the way), and knowledge of an appropriate alternative response (e.g., prompting practice with booking a rapid antigen test in the event of exposure). In contexts where implicit cognition or habit explain substantive variance in a desired behavior, strategies that enhance habits or cue-dependent responding are appropriate. These might include habit-forming strategies such as repetition of the behavior in stable contexts and providing reinforcement for successful behavioral performance.

In the context of COVID-19 preventive behaviors, studies integrating constructs from dual-process models have provided preliminary evidence to indicate the relevance of automatic processes in predicting behavior and elucidating the mechanisms involved. For example, research adopting prospective and longitudinal designs has indicated that habits account for unique variance in physical distancing behavior in two national samples, effects which hold when accounting for past behavior ( Hagger, Smith, et al., 2020 ; Hagger et al., 2021 ). Importantly, these results also indicate, unsurpisingly, that there is substantive stability in habits across three time points, and consistency in their effects on behavior unmediated by intentions, congruent with proposals of dual-process theories. However, it must be stressed that effect sizes for the habit effects in these samples and for this behavior were relatively modest, particularly relative to effects of intentions and the other social cognition constructs. To speculate, it may be that physical distancing requirements vary from context to context, which means the cues to this behavior may not be sufficiently consistent, necessitating reasoned consideration on the part of the actor when making decisions to act in future. It is also a relatively ‘new’ behavior for many, so there may have been less opportunity for many to form habits, especially given time to form a habit varies considerably across individuals ( Lally, van Jaarsveld, Potts, & Wardle, 2010 ). Research is needed to explore potential moderators of habit effects in research on physical distancing in the context of COVID-19, such as the extent to which individuals have had the opportunity to practice physical distancing in the same context. More broadly, research is needed to corroborate habit effects for other COVID-19 preventive behaviors.

4. Mechanisms of action

While researchers have long recognized the value of theory in informing behavior change interventions, many interventions do not have a clear basis in theory despite claims to the contrary by intervention developers (e.g., Michie et al., 2018 ). A major issue for these interventions is the lack of systematic mapping of theoretical principles on to the content and components of the intervention resulting in ‘theory inspired’ rather than ‘theory based’ interventions ( Michie et al., 2018 ). Advances in the scientific study of behavior change has sought to formalize the processes of development and subsequent description of the theoretical constructs targeted in behavioral interventions, the strategies or techniques that form the content of the intervention and how it is delivered ( Kok et al., 2016 ; Michie et al., 2013 ), and the links between them that specify how the intervention ‘works’, that is, the process or mechanism by which the intervention acts to change behavior ( Rothman et al., 2020 ; Sheeran et al., 2017 ). To this end, researchers have used expert consensus and reviews of the extant literature on behavioral interventions to develop organized characterizations or taxonomies of behavior change techniques, and specified links between techniques from taxonomies and the psychological constructs, many originating from social cognition theories, they are purported to change or activate in order to change behavior, known as mechanisms of action ( Avishai et al., 2021 ; Hagger, Moyers, et al., 2020 ; Rothman et al., 2020 ; Sheeran et al., 2017 ). Research is beginning to provide indication of associations between the identified behavior change techniques and theory-based constructs implicated in their mechanisms of action through expert consensus and reviews of stated associations in the extant literature ( Carey et al., 2019 ; Connell et al., 2019 ). There is also research that has identified how theory-based techniques targeting change in social cognition constructs change intention and behavior ( Knittle et al., 2018 ; Sheeran et al., 2016 ). Together this work has advanced knowledge by providing a common, formal nomenclature to describe the techniques that form the content of behavioral interventions, and enabled researchers to better specify how interventions work in changing behavior through change or activation of the theory-based constructs involved. Importantly, it has highlighted the value of developing interventions based on theory, the importance of formal specification of the mechanism of action involved, and facilitated greater clarity and precision in intervention description that enables more effective research syntheses and conceptual replications of interventions.

Providing strong evidence to support the mechanisms of action of behavioral interventions necessitates randomized controlled trials or experimental research designs that demonstrate the effect of behavior change techniques that form the content of the interventions on change in the target theoretical constructs and concomitant change in behavior ( Hagger, Moyers, et al., 2020 ; Rothman et al., 2020 ; Sheeran et al., 2017 ). Verification is derived from testing the mediation of the effect of the intervention on behavior change by change in the theoretical construct. This test of a mechanism of action is illustrated in Fig. 1 ( Hagger, 2019 ). The effect of the intervention on change in the theoretical construct (path a , Fig. 1 ) and the effect of change in the construct on behavior change (path b , Fig. 1 ) constitutes the indirect or mediated effect of the intervention on behavior that should fully or, at least, partially account for the direct effect (path c , Fig. 1 ) on behavior. From an analytic perspective, the mediated effect is tested, the residual effect of the intervention on behavior (path c’ , Fig. 1 ) should be attenuated to zero for full or complete mediation, or to a significantly smaller value for partial mediation. Such tests provide clear evidence in support of the proposed mechanism of action of a behavior change intervention. It is important to note that the change in the mediator should occur in advance of the changes in the behavior, otherwise the mediator may just signal or be broadly indicative of the mechanism ( Kazdin, 2007 ). Testing for the effects of extraneous moderators, such as interpersonal or intrapersonal characteristics, or contextual or environmental variables, of the mediated effect is also indicated by these tests, and would elucidate the extent to which the mechanism accounts for behavior change across such contexts. It is also important that such tests utilize factorial designs that enable isolation of the effects of specific techniques and their mechanisms, which would resolve a perennial problem in cases where interventions use multiple techniques in a single intervention.

Fig. 1

Diagram of a behavior change mechanism of action (Hagger, M. S. (2019). Basic model of a behavior change mechanism of action. PsyArXiv . https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9a5k6 ).

A proliferating body of research has conducted formal tests of the mechanism of action of behavioral interventions adopting theory-based behavior change techniques to change behavior. For example, studies have demonstrated that interventions adopting randomized controlled designs and applying techniques targeting change in attitudes ( Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2005 ), social support ( Quaresma, Palmeira, Martins, Minderico, & Sardinha, 2014 ), and self-efficacy ( Larsen et al., 2021 ) changed physical activity behavior in the target populations through the mediation of change in the measures of the targeted constructs. However, such tests are not routinely conducted – a recent set of meta-reviews indicated that evidence supporting mechanisms of action using mediation tests is sparse and has not advanced significantly over the years ( Suls et al., 2020 ). Meta-analytic research syntheses of behavioral intervention studies have offered some insight demonstrating that interventions that change constructs from social cognition theories, such as attitudes, subjective norms, and self-efficacy, also result in concomitant change in behavior ( Sheeran et al., 2021 ; Webb & Sheeran, 2006 ). However, the change observed using these meta-analytic designs still do not provide definitive evidence for mediation. Alternative approaches to research synthesis have formally tested mediation demonstrating indirect intervention effects on behavior through theory-based constructs across studies ( Rhodes, Boudreau, Weman Josefsson, & Ivarsson, 2020 ; Sheeran et al., 2020 ). However, these syntheses are still sub-optimal means to test the mechanisms as the synthesized data did not provide evidence for the effect of change in the theoretical constructs on change in behavior, both as a result of the intervention, so that association may still be affected by extraneous variables.

Specifying the theoretical basis for behavioral interventions and testing their mechanisms of action in the context of COVID-19 preventive behaviors holds considerable promise in advancing knowledge on how interventions work and their breadth of application. Such an approach together with judicious testing of contextual attributes such as location and target population may provide important evidence of the contexts in which interventions targeting social cognition constructs, such as attitudes and risk perceptions, are likely to have optimal efficacy. However, although a small number of randomized controlled designed interventions adopting techniques aimed at changing behavior through changes in social cognition theory constructs have been reported (e.g., Ahn et al., 2021 ; Keller et al., 2021 ; Okuhara et al., 2020 ; Smith et al., 2021 ), none have tested mechanisms of action using the proposed mediated effects. Given the paucity of this research we recommend the need for interventions based on social cognition theories that test mechanisms of action. Such interventions should using optimal methods including randomized controlled designs and sufficient measures to model behavior change through putative mediators of intervention techniques. These interventions should also be pre-registered consistent with open science guidelines ( Hagger, 2022 ).

5. Future directions

In previous sections we outlined the value of application of social cognition theories in the prediction of COVID-19 preventive behaviors, and their utility in informing behavioral interventions that are optimally efficacious in changing these behaviors. We provided an overview of the current state of the research literature applying these theories in this contexts, and, along the way, highlighted some important avenues for future research, including the need for predictive studies adopting cross-lagged panel and experimental designs to better infer directionality and causality in theory effects; the need for more research testing the prediction of social cognition theories that integrate additional salient processes from other theories to provide comprehensive, optimally parsimonious predictions of these behaviors; and the need for more theory-based interventions testing effects of specific behavior change techniques with formal mediation analyses to test their mechanisms of action. Next, we outline additional research directions that will make important contributions to knowledge on the determinants of preventive behaviors as the global COVID-19 pandemic evolves, and will provide important formative knowledge to inform ongoing intervention and messaging efforts toward the pandemic ‘endgame’ and a shift toward COVID-19 as an endemic illness with threat levels commensurate with other endemic viruses like influenza and the common cold.

5.1. Moving from pandemic to endemic management of COVID-19

There is a need for data on application of social cognition theories to predict preventive behaviors under conditions of endemic management of COVID-19. The theories may provide insight into the determinants of preventive behaviors in isolated outbreaks, short-term ‘circuit breaker’ lockdown measures, and ongoing ‘booster’ vaccination, measures that are likely needed in the foreseeable future to manage the infections. Unsurprisingly, research to date has focused exclusively on preventive behaviors under global pandemic conditions, and ongoing waves of infection including breakthrough infections among the vaccinated population suggest that the pandemic is currently far from over. Continuing research focusing on social cognition determinants of preventive behaviors under current conditions, therefore, has immediate value and is essential to ongoing management. However, preparation for long-term management of COVID-19 infections in the transition from pandemic to endemic conditions needs an evidence base. This can be provided through studies in which hypothetical future COVID-19 conditions are proposed (e.g., the advent of a localized outbreak) followed by measures of social cognition beliefs and intentions to engage in preventive behaviors as a response. Such research may inform messaging interventions relevant to ongoing infection management as pandemic-level infection rates subside and are replaced by isolated outbreaks and seasonal waves.

With the recognition that the immunity afforded by COVID-19 vaccines wanes over time ( Naaber et al., 2021 ), governmental health agencies have approved and recommended the administration of an additional vaccine dose to boost immunity ( CDC, 2021a ; JVCI, 2021 ). These ‘booster’ vaccines are likely to become an ongoing requirement to maintain immunity and minimize infection transmission, particularly to those who are most vulnerable to serious bouts of the illness ( Krause et al., 2021 ). Social cognition theories may offer insight into the determinants likely associated with the uptake of these ‘booster’ vaccine doses, with a view to informing public health messages aimed at promoting adherence to booster vaccine recommendations. The theories may contribute to identification of the beliefs salient to booster vaccine intentions and behavior, such as apathy, fatigue, and decreased perceptions of vulnerability and severity, which may lead the previously vaccinated to fail to get a booster vaccine ( Hagger & Hamilton, 2022 ). Identification of these beliefs may signal potential strategies to counter such perceptions.

5.2. Social cognition, health disparities, and COVID-19

From the onsetof the pandemic, studies have demonstrated considerable disparities in COVID-19 infection rates and outcomes related to the illness including serious cases, ‘long COVID’, and mortality rates in minority groups, particularly racial and ethnic groups that have historically been underserved and those from low incomes and educational backgrounds ( CDC, 2020 ). There is also evidence to suggest disparities in COVID-19 preventive behaviors in these groups, particularly for vaccination ( Ndugga, Hill, Artiga, & Haldar, 2021 ). Recent research has suggested that such behavioral disparities in health contexts may be manifested in the social cognition constructs that line up these behaviors. For example, studies have indicated that constructs from social cognition theories mediate the associations between socio-structural variables that indicate health disparities (e.g., race and ethnicity, income, education) on health behavior participation ( Hagger & Hamilton, 2021 ; Orbell, Szczepura, Weller, Gumber, & Hagger, 2017 ). These data suggest that individuals from underserved minority groups, on low incomes, or with lower levels of education may be less likely to view illnesses and other risky conditions as threatening, and report lower self-efficacy with respect to health behaviors, which manifests in lower participation in health behaviors. Furthermore, these socio-structural variables also moderate relations among constructs and behavior in social cognition theories ( Schüz, Brick, Wilding, & Conner, 2020 ). For example, individuals from low income and education backgrounds are less likely to act on their intentions, which may be the result of lower expectation of control over their behavior due to experiences of healthcare inaccessibility or disenfranchisement from healthcare services. Recent research has corroborated these moderating effects in eight COVID-19 preventive behaviors including physical distancing, restricting time outside the home, and wearing facemasks ( Schüz et al., 2021 ). Larger intention-COVID-19 preventive behavior relations were observed in less socio-economically deprived groups. The research provides preliminary information on the mechanisms to which observed disparities in preventive behaviors could be attributed. It may also signal potential targets for interventions that may be effective in promoting behavior change in these groups when the source of disparities are difficult to modify or change in the short term.

5.3. Traits in social cognition theories

While there is emerging research examining effects of traits or trait-like constructs such as personality and other intrapersonal individual difference constructs on COVID-19 preventive behaviors (e.g., Nofal, Cacciotti, & Lee, 2020 ), there is little research that has explored the effects of these dispositional constructs in the context of social cognition theories. Incorporating trait-like constructs into social cognition theories may indicate mechanistic explanations for how these dispositions relate to behavior. A central premise of many social cognition theories is that trait-like constructs serve as sources of information in the decision-making process and inform individuals’ beliefs with respect to performing a target behavior in future ( Ajzen, 1991 ; Bandura, 1986 ). Empirically, therefore, social cognition constructs should act as mediators of effects of dispositions on behavior. Accordingly, verifying that such constructs serve as distal correlates of behavior, will provide potential information on the constructs that should be targeted to change behavior, particularly among groups with specific traits. Tests of these proposed mediation effects in health contexts abound. For example, researchers have demonstrated that dispositional constructs that reflect better capacity to pursue goal-directed behaviors, such as the conscientiousness personality trait and trait self-control, are associated with greater participation in health behavior mediated by social cognition constructs such as attitudes and self-efficacy (e.g., Bogg, 2008 ; Conner & Abraham, 2001 ; Hagger et al., 2019 ).

The mediation of traits on behavior in social cognition theories has been tested in the context of COVID-19 preventive behaviors. As an illustrative example, personality traits openness to experience and agreeableness personality traits on adherence to COVID-19 prevention guidelines were mediated by attitudes, and both attitudes and perceived norms, respectively ( Bogg & Milad, 2020 ). A finding that suggests that individuals with tendencies to be more socially agreeable and open-minded are more likely to hold beliefs in the utility of COVID-19 preventive behaviors, and view their social environments as supportive, which may be implicated in their decisions to engage in those behaviors. Analogously, highly politicized context of COVID-19 prevention, political orientation, a summary of individuals' political beliefs as liberal or conservative, is a trait-like construct that would be expected to affect individuals' decision making with respect to COVID-19 preventive behaviors. For example, studies have indicated that political orientation has been shown to be associated with COVID-19 vaccine intentions, with conservatives less likely to intend to perform behaviors such as mask wearing and getting vaccinated (e.g., Hamilton & Hagger, 2022 ; Huynh, Zsila, & Martinez-Berman, 2022 ). Further, effects of political orientation on booster vaccine intentions has been shown to be fully mediated by social cognition constructs (attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, risk perceptions), with more conservative beliefs related to lower intentions ( Hagger & Hamilton, 2022 ). These findings highlight the importance of considering political beliefs as a trait that informs individuals’ estimates of their intentions in this context. Incorporating measures of political orientation in social cognition theories may, therefore, assist in explaining the mechanisms underpinning these effects.

A further, seldom-investigated role for traits or trait-like constructs in social cognition theories is their role in moderating the effects of social cognition constructs on intention and behaviors. Traits may magnify or diminish effects of key social cognition constructs on intention and behavior, and may, therefore, be directly implicated in the decision-making process. For example, research indicates that conscientiousness and extraversion personality traits moderate the intention-behavior relationship in the domain of physical activity, with larger intention-behavior relations observed at higher levels of these traits ( Rhodes, Courneya, & Hayduk, 2002 ). This finding indicates that individuals with greater work ethic and those who have greater tendency to explore new opportunities tend to be more effective in acting on their exercise intentions. Similarly, research demonstrates that conscientiousness moderates the effect of affective attitudes on intention, such that the affective attitude-intention relationship was smaller among those reporting high conscientiousness ( Rhodes et al., 2002 ). This suggests that individuals scoring higher on conscientiousness are less likely to base their intentions on a consideration of affective, impulse-related outcomes (e.g., “exercise makes me feel good) and more likely to base them on cognitive, utility-based outcomes (e.g., “exercise will make me fitter”).

Numerous traits have been explored as determinants of COVID-19 preventive behaviors, including personality constructs ( Zettler et al., 2022 ) and need for cognition ( Xu & Cheng, 2021 ), but relatively few as moderators of intention-behavior relations or effects of social cognition constructs on intentions. As in the mediation analyses reported previously, political orientation would be expected to impact individuals’ decision making in the context of COVID-19 preventive behaviors. As an illustration, political orientation has been shown to moderate effects of perceived determining factors like catastrophic potential, dread, and moral nature on risk perceptions, a social cognition construct ( Ju & You, 2022 ). However, there has been few investigations examining political orientation as a moderator within social cognition models, particularly its role as a moderator of the intention-behavior relationship. To speculate, individuals with more conservative political beliefs may not only be less likely to form intentions to perform behaviors viewed as overly-restrictive, or associated with government overreach, such as face mask wearing, but may also be less likely to act on their intentions because their beliefs may place limits on their desire to act non-normatively. As such, political orientation would be expected to moderate the intention-behavior relationship. We look to future research to explore such effects.

Taken together, knowledge that traits and enduring beliefs such as personality and political orientation are both associated with individuals’ specific beliefs regarding performing COVID-19 preventive behaviors, and may moderate their effects on behavior, highlights the need for tailored interventions, or dedicated messages, aimed at countering or challenging those behavioral beliefs. Future research should seek to verify these predictions and test the efficacy of messaging aimed at promoting COVID-19 preventive behaviors in groups defined by specific dispositional characteristics.

5.4. Affective processes

Incorporation of constructs that represent affective processes in social cognition theories has made an important contribution to explaining variance in health behavior. For example, numerous studies have incorporated constructs representing anticipated affect, including the affective component of attitude and constructs such as anticipated regret and negative emotion, into predictive tests of these theories in health behavior contexts (e.g., Rivis, Sheeran, & Armitage, 2009 ). Generally, anticipated affective responses have been shown to predict behavior via the mediation of intentions, but also directly, suggesting that affective considerations not only inform decision making, but may also lead to impulsive or automatic behavioral engagement (e.g., Conner, McEachan, Taylor, O'Hara, & Lawton, 2015 ). Exploring effects of these constructs may also provide salient information on the determinants of COVID-19 preventive behaviors. For example, anticipated affect constructs such as regret have been shown to be related to physical distancing behavior, but effects are small and tend to be usurped by other constructs that have greater salience, such as moral norms ( Hagger, Smith, et al., 2020 ). Similarly, fear and worry of COVID-19 infection has also been shown to predict intentions to perform COVID-19 preventive behaviors ( Coifman et al., 2021 ), and have been shown to be mediated by constructs from the theory of planned behavior ( Yahaghi et al., 2021 ). In such cases, fear perceptions serve as an information source in decision making toward these preventive behaviors, a process consistent with the premise from social cognition theories that threat perceptions are likely to compel individuals to select a course of behavior to manage the threat.

These findings notwithstanding, there is a relative dearth of research examining affective processes in the context of social cognition theories in COVID-19. Such approaches have much potential to inform knowledge given that affective beliefs are likely to be highly salient to making decisions for some COVID-19 preventive behaviors, such as vaccination ( Chou & Budenz, 2020 ). Similarly, researchers should also consider examining the moderating conditions that determine whether affective beliefs, such as affective attitudes, are likely to be most salient when predicting intentions to perform these behaviors alongside utilitarian beliefs, such as cognitive attitudes. Such research may provide further insight into the relative contribution of affective processes in decision making, and a basis for the development of targeted messages in behavior change interventions that promote engagement in preventive COVID-19 behaviors.

6. Conclusion

Minimizing the transmission of COVID-19 infections requires widespread adherence to COVID-19 preventive behaviors that include mitigation measures (e.g., physical distancing, wearing face coverings, avoiding gathering in large groups, engaging in sanitization behaviors, participating in rapid testing) and immunization through vaccination. Adherence, however, is sub-optimal for many of these behaviors, necessitating health authorities to intervene to promote uptake and maintenance of these behaviors going forward. Such behavior change interventions should be informed by theory and evidence derived from behavioral science. Social cognition theories have made substantive contributions to the identification of the belief-based determinants of health behavior (e.g., attitudes, norms, self-efficacy, risk perceptions). In addition, research based on these theories has led to the specification of links between theoretical constructs and behavior change techniques purported to change them, known as mechanisms of action. Application of social cognition theories in the context of COVID-19 has identified the salient predictors of multiple preventive behaviors. Furthermore, integrated approaches have also demonstrated the salience of additional constructs, like moral norms and anticipated affect, and processes, such as planning and habits, in accounting for unique variance in these behaviors. Importantly, these data have begun to inform behavior change interventions adopting techniques targeting the social cognition constructs shown to be related to COVID-19 preventive behaviors. However, few interventions in this context have a strong basis in theory, and none have tested theory-based mechanisms of action.

Considering these evidence gaps in research on COVID-19 preventive behaviors, future studies should test social cognition theory predictions using cross-lagged panel, randomized controlled, and experimental designs that better enable inference of directionality and causality. There is also the need to apply these theories to predict emergent preventive behaviors such as booster vaccination, and to the management of COVID-19 as an endemic illness. We also call for research that incorporates socio-structural variables, individual difference constructs, and constructs representing affective processes into existing theories to address key questions on the determinants of COVID-19 preventive behaviors. These recommendations will broaden the contribution that applying social cognition theories makes to an evidence base to inform efficacious and efficient behavior change interventions in the context of COVID-19 prevention.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Martin S. Hagger: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Kyra Hamilton: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Declarations of competing interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest with the work conducted in this study. All views expressed are solely those of the authors. The other authors have no financial disclosures.

  • Adiyoso W., Wilopo Social distancing intentions to reduce the spread of COVID-19: The extended theory of planned behavior. BMC Public Health. 2021; 21 (1):1836. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-11884-5. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ahn J.N., Hu D., Vega M. Changing pace: Using implementation intentions to enhance social distancing behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 2021 doi: 10.1037/xap0000385. No Pagination Specified-No Pagination Specified. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1991; 50 (2):179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ajzen I., Fishbein M. Prentice Hall; 1980. Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ajzen I., Schmidt P. In: The handbook of behavior change. Hagger M.S., Cameron L.D., Hamilton K., Hankonen N., Lintunen T., editors. Cambridge University Press; 2020. Changing behavior using the theory of planned behavior; pp. 17–31. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Albarracín D. Cambridge University Press; 2021. Action and inaction in a social world: Predicting and changing attitudes and behaviors. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Albarracín D., Johnson B.T., Fishbein M., Muellerleile P.A. Theories of reasoned action and planned behavior as models of condom use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2001; 127 (1):142–161. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.142. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Albarracín D., Jung H. A research agenda for the post-COVID-19 world: Theory and research in social psychology. Asian Journal of Social Psychology. 2021; 24 (1):10–17. doi: 10.1111/ajsp.12469. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Armitage C.J. Evidence that implementation intentions reduce dietary fat intake: A randomized trial. Health Psychology. 2004; 23 :319–323. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Avishai A., Brewer N.T., Mendel J.R., Sheeran P. Expanding the analysis of mechanisms of action in behavioral interventions: Cognitive change versus cognitive activation. Psychology and Health. 2021:1–20. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2021.1969021. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura A. Prentice-Hall; 1986. Social foundations of thought and action: A social-cognitive theory. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bar-On Y.M., Goldberg Y., Mandel M., Bodenheimer O., Freedman L., Kalkstein N., et al. Protection of BNT162b2 vaccine booster against Covid-19 in Israel. New England Journal of Medicine. 2021; 385 (15):1393–1400. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa2114255. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barile J.P., Guerin R.J., Fisher K.A., Tian L.H., Okun A.H., Vanden Esschert K.L., et al. Theory-based behavioral predictors of self-reported use of face coverings in public settings during the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2020 doi: 10.1093/abm/kaaa109. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bartsch S.M., Ferguson M.C., McKinnell J.A., O'Shea K.J., Wedlock P.T., Siegmund S.S., et al. The potential health care costs and resource use associated with COVID-19 in the United States. Health Affairs. 2020; 39 (6):927–935. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2020.00426. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beeckman M., De Paepe A., Van Alboom M., Maes S., Wauters A., Baert F., et al. Adherence to the physical distancing measures during the COVID-19 pandemic: A HAPA-based perspective. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being. 2020; 12 (4):1224–1243. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12242. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bergwerk M., Gonen T., Lustig Y., Amit S., Lipsitch M., Cohen C., et al. Covid-19 breakthrough infections in vaccinated health care workers. New England Journal of Medicine. 2021; 385 (16):1474–1484. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa2109072. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Betsch C., Korn L., Burgard T., Gaissmaier W., Felgendreff L., Eitze S., et al. The four weeks before lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany: A weekly serial cross-sectional survey on risk perceptions, knowledge, public trust and behaviour, 3 to 25 March 2020. Euro Surveillance. 2021; 26 (42) doi: 10.2807/1560-7917.ES.2021.26.42.2001900. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bieleke M., Martarelli C.S., Wolff W. If-then planning, self-control, and boredom as predictors of adherence to social distancing guidelines: Evidence from a two-wave longitudinal study with a behavioral intervention. Current Psychology. 2021 doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02106-7. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Block J. A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychological Bulletin. 1995; 117 :187–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.187. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bogg T. Conscientiousness, the transtheoretical model of change, and exercise: A neo-socioanalytic integration of trait and social-cognitive frameworks in the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality. 2008; 76 (4):775–802. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00504.x. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bogg T., Milad E. Demographic, personality, and social cognition correlates of coronavirus guideline adherence in a U.S. sample. Health Psychology. 2020; 39 (12):1026–1036. doi: 10.1037/hea0000891. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bonell C., Michie S., Reicher S., West R., Bear L., Yardley L., et al. Harnessing behavioural science in public health campaigns to maintain ‘social distancing’ in response to the COVID-19 pandemic: Key principles. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2020; 74 :617–619. doi: 10.1136/jech-2020-214290. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brown D.J., Hagger M.S., Hamilton K. The mediating role of constructs representing reasoned-action and automatic processes on the past behavior-future behavior relationship. Social Science & Medicine. 2020; 258 doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113085. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Bruijn G.-J., Rhodes R.E., van Osch L. Does action planning moderate the intention-habit interaction in the exercise domain? A three-way interaction analysis investigation. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2012; 35 (5):509–519. doi: 10.1007/s10865-011-9380-2. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Capasso M., Caso D., Conner M. Anticipating pride or regret? Effects of anticipated affect focused persuasive messages on intention to get vaccinated against COVID-19. Social Science & Medicine. 2021; 289 doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114416. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carey R.N., Connell L.E., Johnston M., Rothman A.J., de Bruin M., Kelly M.P., et al. Behavior change techniques and their mechanisms of action: A synthesis of links described in published intervention literature. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2019; 53 (8):693–707. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay078. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carpenter C.J. A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of health belief model variables in predicting behavior. Health Communication. 2010; 25 (8):661–669. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2010.521906. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • CDC . 2020. COVID-19 in racial and ethnic minority groups. https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/need-extra-precautions/racial-ethnic-minorities.html from. [ Google Scholar ]
  • CDC . 2021. CDC expands eligibility for COVID-19 booster shots to all adults. https://www.cdc.gov/media/releases/2021/s1119-booster-shots.html from. [ Google Scholar ]
  • CDC . 2021. Excess deaths associated with COVID-19. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nvss/vsrr/covid19/excess_deaths.htm from. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chatzisarantis N.L.D., Hagger M.S. Effects of a brief intervention based on the theory of planned behavior on leisure time physical activity participation. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology. 2005; 27 :470–487. doi: 10.1123/jsep.27.4.470. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chatzisarantis N.L.D., Hagger M.S., Smith B., Phoenix C. The influences of continuation intentions on the execution of social behaviour within the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2004; 43 (4):551–583. doi: 10.1348/0144666042565399. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chou W.-Y.S., Budenz A. Considering emotion in COVID-19 vaccine communication: Addressing vaccine hesitancy and fostering vaccine confidence. Health Communication. 2020; 35 (14):1718–1722. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2020.1838096. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chu H., Liu S. Integrating health behavior theories to predict American's intention to receive a COVID-19 vaccine. Patient Education and Counseling. 2021; 104 (8):1878–1886. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2021.02.031. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clark C., Davila A., Regis M., Kraus S. Predictors of COVID-19 voluntary compliance behaviors: An international investigation. Global Transitions. 2020; 2 :76–82. doi: 10.1016/j.glt.2020.06.003. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Coifman K.G., Disabato D.J., Aurora P., Seah T.H.S., Mitchell B., Simonovic N., et al. What drives preventive health behavior during a global pandemic? Emotion and worry. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2021; 55 (8):791–804. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaab048. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Connell L.E., Carey R.N., de Bruin M., Rothman A.J., Johnston M., Kelly M.P., et al. Links between behavior change techniques and mechanisms of action: An expert consensus study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2019; 53 (8):708–720. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay082. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conner M.T., Abraham C. Conscientiousness and the theory of planned behavior: Toward a more complete model of the antecedents of intentions and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2001; 27 (11):1547–1561. doi: 10.1177/01461672012711014. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conner M.T., McEachan R., Taylor N., O'Hara J., Lawton R. Role of affective attitudes and anticipated affective reactions in predicting health behaviors. Health Psychology. 2015; 34 (6):642–652. doi: 10.1037/hea0000143. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conner M.T., Norman P. In: Predicting and changing health behaviour: Research and practice with social cognition models. 3rd ed. Conner M.T., Norman P., editors. Open University Press; 2015. Predicting and changing health behaviour: A social cognition approach; pp. 1–29. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conner M.T., Norman P. 3rd ed. Open University Press; 2015. Predicting and changing health behaviour: Research and practice with social cognition models. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cooke R., Sheeran P. Moderation of cognition-intention and cognition-behaviour relations: A meta-analysis of properties of variables from the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2004; 43 (2):159–186. doi: 10.1348/0144666041501688. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crozier A., Rajan S., Buchan I., McKee M. Put to the test: Ase of rapid testing technologies for covid-19. BMJ. 2021; 372 :n208. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n208. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Das A.K., Abdul Kader Jilani M.M., Uddin M.S., Uddin M.A., Ghosh A.K. Fighting ahead: Adoption of social distancing in COVID-19 outbreak through the lens of theory of planned behavior. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment. 2021; 31 (1–4):373–393. doi: 10.1080/10911359.2020.1833804. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Davis R., Campbell R., Hildon Z., Hobbs L., Michie S. Theories of behaviour and behaviour change across the social and behavioural sciences: A scoping review. Health Psychology Review. 2015; 9 (3):323–344. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.941722. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Derksen C., Keller F.M., Lippke S. Obstetric healthcare workers' adherence to hand hygiene recommendations during the COVID-19 pandemic: Observations and social-cognitive determinants. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being. 2020; 12 (4):1286–1305. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12240. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Divine A., Berry T., Rodgers W., Hall C. The relationship of self-efficacy and explicit and implicit associations on the intention–behavior gap. Journal of Physical Activity and Health. 2021; 18 (1):29–36. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2019-0033. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • DuCharme K.A., Brawley L.R. Predicting the intentions and behavior of exercise initiates using two forms of self-efficacy. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1995; 18 :479–497. doi: 10.1007/BF01904775. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duckworth A.L., Gendler T.S., Gross J.J. Situational strategies for self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2016; 11 (1):35–55. doi: 10.1177/1745691615623247. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fishbein M., Ajzen I. Psychology Press; 2010. Predicting and changing behavior: The reasoned action approach. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fishbein M., Triandis H.C., Kanfer F.H., Becker M., Middlestadt S.E., Eichler A. In: Handbook of health psychology. Baum A., Revenson T.A., Singer J.E., editors. Lawrence Erlbaum; 2001. Factors influencing behavior and behavior change; pp. 3–17. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gardner B., Lally P., Rebar A.L. Does habit weaken the relationship between intention and behaviour? Revisiting the habit-intention interaction hypothesis. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2020; 14 (8) doi: 10.1111/spc3.12553. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gardner B., Lally P., Wardle J. Making health habitual: The psychology of ‘habit-formation’ and general practice. British Journal of General Practice. 2012; 62 (605):664. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gardner B., Rebar A., Lally P. In: The handbook of behavior change. Hagger M.S., Cameron L.D., Hamilton K., Hankonen N., Lintunen T., editors. Cambridge University Press; 2020. Habit interventions; pp. 599–616. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gibbons F.X., Gerrard M., Blanton H., Russell D.W. Reasoned action and social reaction: Willingness and intention as independent predictors of health risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998; 74 :1164–1180. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gibson L.P., Magnan R.E., Kramer E.B., Bryan A.D. Theory of planned behavior analysis of social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic: Focusing on the intention–behavior gap. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2021; 55 (8):805–812. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaab041. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gollob H.F., Reichardt C.S. Taking account of time lags in causal models. Child Development. 1987; 58 :80–92. doi: 10.2307/1130293. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gollwitzer P.M. Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist. 1999; 54 (7):493–503. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gollwitzer P.M., Sheeran P. Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 2006; 38 :69–119. doi: 10.1013/S0065-2601(06)38002-1. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S. Theoretical integration in health psychology: Unifying ideas and complimentary explanations. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2009; 14 (2):189–194. doi: 10.1348/135910708X397034. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S. Avoiding the ‘déjà-variable’ phenomenon: Social psychology needs more guides to constructs. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014; 5 :52. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00052. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S. Non-conscious processes and dual-process theories in health psychology. Health Psychology Review. 2016; 10 (4):375–380. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2016.1244647. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S. Basic model of a behavior change mechanism of action. 2019. PsyArXiv . [ CrossRef ]
  • Hagger M.S. Developing an open science ‘mindset. Health Psychology and Behavioral Medicine. 2022; 10 (1):1–21. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2021.2012474. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Cameron L.D., Hamilton K., Hankonen N., Lintunen T., editors. The handbook of behavior change. Cambridge University Press; 2020. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Cheung M.W.L., Ajzen I., Hamilton K. Perceived behavioral control moderating effects in the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology. 2022; 41 (2):155–167. doi: 10.1037/hea0001153. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Gucciardi D.F., Chatzisarantis N.L.D. On nomological validity and auxiliary assumptions: The importance of simultaneously testing effects in social cognitive theories applied to health behavior and some guidelines. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017; 8 :1933. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01933. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Hamilton K. In: The handbook of behavior change. Hagger M.S., Cameron L.D., Hamilton K., Hankonen N., Lintunen T., editors. Cambridge University Press; 2020. Changing behavior using integrated theories; pp. 208–224. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Hamilton K. Effects of socio-structural variables in the theory of planned behavior: A mediation model in multiple samples and behaviors. Psychology and Health. 2021; 36 (3):307–333. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2020.1784420. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Hamilton K. Predicting COVID-19 booster vaccine intentions. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being. 2022 doi: 10.1111/aphw.12349. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Hankonen N., Kangro E.-M., Lintunen T., Pagaduan J., Polet J., et al. Trait self-control, social cognition constructs, and intentions: Correlational evidence for mediation and moderation effects in diverse health behaviors. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being. 2019; 11 (3):407–437. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12153. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Lonsdale A., Koka A., Hein V., Pasi H., Lintunen T., et al. An intervention to reduce alcohol consumption in undergraduate students using implementation intentions and mental simulations: A cross-national study. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2012; 19 (1):82–96. doi: 10.1007/s12529-011-9163-8. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Moyers S., McAnally K., McKinley L.E. Known knowns and known unknowns on behavior change interventions and mechanisms of action. Health Psychology Review. 2020; 14 (1):199–212. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2020.1719184. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Orbell S. The common sense model of illness self-regulation: A conceptual review and proposed extended model. Health Psychology Review. 2021 doi: 10.1080/17437199.2021.1878050. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Polet J., Lintunen T. The reasoned action approach applied to health behavior: Role of past behavior and test of some key moderators using meta-analytic structural equation modeling. Social Science & Medicine. 2018; 213 :85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.07.038. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Smith S.R., Keech J.J., Moyers S.A., Hamilton K. Predicting social distancing intention and behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic: An integrated social cognition model. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2020; 54 (10):713–727. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaaa073. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Smith S.R., Keech J.J., Moyers S.A., Hamilton K. Predicting physical distancing over time during COVID-19: Testing an integrated model. Psychology and Health. 2021 doi: 10.1080/08870446.2021.1968397. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Trost N., Keech J., Chan D.K.C., Hamilton K. Predicting sugar consumption: Application of an integrated dual-process, dual-phase model. Appetite. 2017; 116 :147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.04.032. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagger M.S., Weed M.E. Debate: Do behavioral interventions work in the real world? International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2019; 16 :36. doi: 10.1186/s12966-019-0795-4. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hall P.A., Fong G.T. Temporal self-regulation theory: A model for individual health behavior. Health Psychology Review. 2007; 1 (1):6–52. doi: 10.1080/17437190701492437. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamilton K., Gibbs I., Keech J.J., Hagger M.S. Reasoned and implicit processes in heavy episodic drinking: An integrated dual process model. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2020; 25 (1):189–209. doi: 10.1111/BJHP.12401. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamilton K., Hagger M.S. The vaccination concerns in COVID-19 scale (VaCCS): Development and validation. PLoS One. 2022; 17 (3) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0264784. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamilton K., Johnson B.T. In: The handbook of behavior change. Hagger M.S., Cameron L.D., Hamilton K., Hankonen N., Lintunen T., editors. Cambridge University Press; 2020. Attitude and persuasive communication interventions; pp. 445–460. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamilton K., Kirkpatrick A., Rebar A., Hagger M.S. Child sun safety: Application of an integrated behavior change model. Health Psychology. 2017; 36 (9):916–926. doi: 10.1037/hea0000533. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamilton K., Smith S.R., Keech J.J., Moyers S.A., Hagger M.S. Application of the health action process approach to social distancing behavior during COVID‐19. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being. 2020; 12 (4):1244–1269. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12231. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamilton K., van Dongen A., Hagger M.S. An extended theory of planned behavior for parent-for-child health behaviors: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology. 2020; 39 (10):863–878. doi: 10.1037/hea0000940. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Harvey A.G., Armstrong C.C., Callaway C.A., Gumport N.B., Gasperetti C.E. COVID-19 prevention via the science of habit formation. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2021; 30 (2):174–180. doi: 10.1177/0963721421992028. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heckhausen H., Gollwitzer P.M. Thought contents and cognitive functioning in motivational and volitional states of mind. Motivation and Emotion. 1987; 11 :101–120. doi: 10.1007/BF00992338. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holmes E.A., O'Connor R.C., Perry V.H., Tracey I., Wessely S., Arseneault L., et al.… Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: A call for action for mental health science. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2020; 7 (6):547–560. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168-1. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Huynh H.P., Zsila Á, Martinez-Berman L. Psychosocial predictors of intention to vaccinate against the coronavirus (COVID-19) Behavioral Medicine. 2022 doi: 10.1080/08964289.2021.1990006. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Imai K., Keele L., Tingley D., Yamamoto T. Unpacking the black box of causality: Learning about causal mechanisms from experimental and observational studies. American Political Science Review. 2011; 105 (4):765–789. doi: 10.2307/23275352. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Irfan M., Akhtar N., Ahmad M., Shahzad F., Elavarasan R.M., Wu H., et al. Assessing public willingness to wear face masks during the COVID-19 pandemic: Fresh insights from the theory of planned behavior. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18 (9):4577. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jackson J.K., Weiss M.A., Schwarzenberg A.B., Nelson R.M., Sutter K.M., Sutherland M.D. Congressional Research Services; Washington DC: 2021. Economic effects of COVID-19. from https://sgp.fas.org/crs/row/R46270.pdf . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jacobs N., Hagger M.S., Streukens S., De Bourdeaudhuij I., Claes N. Testing an integrated model of the theory of planned behaviour and self-determination theory for different energy-balance related behaviours and intervention intensities. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2011; 16 (1):113–134. doi: 10.1348/135910710X519305. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jang D., Kim I., Kwon S. Motivation and intention toward physical activity during the COVID-19 pandemic: Perspectives from integrated model of self-determination and planned behavior theories. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021; 12 :3124. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.714865. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ju Y., You M. It's politics, isn't it? Investigating direct and indirect influences of political orientation on risk perception of COVID-19. Risk Analysis. 2022; 42 (1):56–68. doi: 10.1111/risa.13801. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • JVCI . Department of Health and Social Care; UK: 2021. JCVI statement, september 2021: COVID-19 booster vaccine programme for winter 2021 to 2022. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/jcvi-statement-september-2021-covid-19-booster-vaccine-programme-for-winter-2021-to-2022 Retrieved November 19 from. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kashte S., Gulbake A., El-Amin S.F., Iii, Gupta A. COVID-19 vaccines: Rapid development, implications, challenges and future prospects. Human Cell. 2021; 34 (3):711–733. doi: 10.1007/s13577-021-00512-4. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kazdin A.E. Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2007; 3 (1):1–27. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Keatley D.A., Clarke D.D., Hagger M.S. Investigating the predictive validity of implicit and explicit measures of motivation on condom use, physical activity, and healthy eating. Psychology and Health. 2012; 27 (5):550–569. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2011.605451. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Keller J., Kwasnicka D., Wilhelm L.O., Lorbeer N., Pauly T., Domke A., et al. Hand washing and related cognitions following a brief behavior change intervention during the COVID-19 pandemic: A pre-post analysis. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2021 doi: 10.1007/s12529-021-10042-w. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kelley T.L. World Book Co; 1927. Interpretation of educational measurements. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knittle K., Nurmi J., Crutzen R., Hankonen N., Beattie M., Dombrowski S.U. How can interventions increase motivation for physical activity? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review. 2018; 12 (3):211–230. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1435299. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kojan L., Burbach L., Ziefle M., Calero Valdez A. Perceptions of behaviour efficacy, not perceptions of threat, are drivers of COVID-19 protective behaviour in Germany. 2021. from. [ CrossRef ]
  • Kok G., Gottlieb N.H., Peters G.-J.Y., Mullen P.D., Parcel G.S., Ruiter R.A.C., et al. A taxonomy of behavior change methods: An intervention mapping approach. Health Psychology Review. 2016; 10 (3):297–312. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2015.1077155. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krause P.R., Fleming T.R., Peto R., Longini I.M., Figueroa J.P., Sterne J.A.C., et al. Considerations in boosting COVID-19 vaccine immune responses. The Lancet. 2021; 398 (10308):1377–1380. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(21)02046-8. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lally P., van Jaarsveld C.H.M., Potts H.W.W., Wardle J. How are habits formed: Modelling habit formation in the real world. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2010; 40 :998–1009. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.674. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lao C.K., Li X., Zhao N., Gou M., Zhou G. Using the health action process approach to predict facemask use and hand washing in the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic in China. Current Psychology. 2021 doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01985-0. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Larsen B., Dunsiger S.I., Pekmezi D., Linke S., Hartman S.J., Marcus B.H. Psychosocial mediators of physical activity change in a web-based intervention for Latinas. Health Psychology. 2021; 40 (1):21–29. doi: 10.1037/hea0001041. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Latkin C., Dayton L.A., Yi G., Konstantopoulos A., Park J., Maulsby C., et al. COVID-19 vaccine intentions in the United States, a social-ecological framework. Vaccine. 2021; 39 (16):2288–2294. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2021.02.058. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leventhal H., Meyer D., Nerenz D. In: Medical psychology. Rachman S., editor. Vol. II. Pergamon Press; 1980. The common sense model of illness danger; pp. 7–30. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lin C.-Y., Imani V., Ghasemi Z., Majd N.R., Griffiths M.D., Hamilton K., et al. Using an integrated social cognition model to predict COVID-19 preventive behaviors. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2020; 25 (4):981–1005. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12465. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liska A.E., Felson R.B., Chamlin M., Baccaglini W. Estimating attitude-behavior reciprocal effects within a theoretical specification. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1984; 47 :15–23. doi: 10.2307/3033884. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luszczynska A., Szczuka Z., Abraham C., Baban A., Brooks S., Cipolletta S., et al.… The interplay between strictness of policies and individuals’ self-regulatory efforts: Associations with handwashing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2021 doi: 10.1093/abm/kaab102. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martiny-Huenger T., Martiny S.E., Parks-Stamm E.J., Pfeiffer E., Gollwitzer P.M. From conscious thought to automatic action: A simulation account of action planning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2017; 146 (10):1513–1525. doi: 10.1037/xge0000344. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mathieu E., Ritchie H., Ortiz-Ospina E., Roser M., Hasell J., Appel C., et al. A global database of COVID-19 vaccinations. Nature Human Behaviour. 2021; 5 (7):947–953. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01122-8. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Matute J., Palau-Saumell R., Meyer J., Derqui B., Jiménez-Asenjo N. Are you getting it? Integrating theories to explain intentions to get vaccinated against COVID-19 in Spain. Journal of Risk Research. 2021:1–20. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2021.1958044. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McEachan R.R.C., Conner M.T., Taylor N., Lawton R.J. Prospective prediction of health-related behaviors with the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review. 2011; 5 (2):97–144. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2010.521684. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McElfish P.A., Purvis R., James L.P., Willis D.E., Andersen J.A. Perceived barriers to COVID-19 testing. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18 (5):2278. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McEwan D., Beauchamp M.R., Kouvousis C., Ray C.M., Wyrough A., Rhodes R.E. Examining the active ingredients of physical activity interventions underpinned by theory versus no stated theory: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review. 2019; 13 (1):1–17. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1547120. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McMillan B., Conner M. In: Cambridge handbook of psychology, health and medicine. 2nd ed. Ayers A.B.S., McManus C., Newman S., Wallston K., Weinman J., West R., editors. Cambridge University Press; 2007. Health cognition assessment; pp. 260–266. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Michie S. 2020. Behavioural strategies for reducing covid-19 transmission in the general population. https://blogs.bmj.com/bmj/2020/03/03/behavioural-strategies-for-reducing-covid-19-transmission-in-the-general-population/ 2020, March 3, from. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Michie S., Carey R.N., Johnston M., Rothman A.J., de Bruin M., Kelly M.P., et al. From theory-inspired to theory-based interventions: A protocol for developing and testing a methodology for linking behaviour change techniques to theoretical mechanisms of action. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2018; 52 (6):501–512. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9816-6. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Michie S., Richardson M., Johnston M., Abraham C., Francis J., Hardeman W., et al. The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: Building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2013; 46 (1):81–95. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Michie S., Rubin G.J., Amlôt R. 2020. Behavioural science must be at the heart of the public health response to COVID-19. https://blogs.bmj.com/bmj/2020/02/28/behavioural-science-must-be-at-the-heart-of-the-public-health-response-to-covid-19/ from. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Michie S., West R., Rogers M.B., Bonell C., Rubin G.J., Amlôt R. Reducing SARS-CoV-2 transmission in the UK: A behavioural science approach to identifying options for increasing adherence to social distancing and shielding vulnerable people. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2020 doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12428. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Milne S., Sheeran P., Orbell S. Prediction and intervention in health-related behavior: A meta-analytic review of protection motivation theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2000; 30 :106–143. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02308.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Montaño D.E., Kasprzyk D. In: Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice. 5th ed. Glanz K., Rimer B.K., Viswanath K., editors. Jossey-Bass; 2015. Theory of reasoned action, theory of planned behavior, and the integrated behavioral model; pp. 95–124. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Naaber P., Tserel L., Kangro K., Sepp E., Jürjenson V., Adamson A., et al. Dynamics of antibody response to BNT162b2 vaccine after six months: A longitudinal prospective study. The Lancet Regional Health - Europe. 2021; 10 doi: 10.1016/j.lanepe.2021.100208. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ndugga N., Hill L., Artiga S., Haldar S. Latest data on COVID-19 vaccinations by race/ethnicity. 2021. https://www.kff.org/coronavirus-covid-19/issue-brief/latest-data-on-covid-19-vaccinations-by-race-ethnicity/ from.
  • Nofal A.M., Cacciotti G., Lee N. Who complies with COVID-19 transmission mitigation behavioral guidelines? PLoS One. 2020; 15 (10) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240396. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Norman P., Cameron D., Epton T., Webb T.L., Harris P.R., Millings A., et al. A randomized controlled trial of a brief online intervention to reduce alcohol consumption in new university students: Combining self-affirmation, theory of planned behaviour messages, and implementation intentions. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2018; 23 (1):108–127. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12277. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Norman P., Wilding S., Conner M.T. Reasoned action approach and compliance with recommended behaviours to prevent the transmission of the SARS-CoV-2 virus in the UK. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2020; 25 (4):1006–1019. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12474. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Okuhara T., Okada H., Kiuchi T. Examining persuasive message type to encourage staying at home during the COVID-19 pandemic and social lockdown: A randomized controlled study in Japan. Patient Education and Counseling. 2020; 103 (12):2588–2593. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2020.08.016. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Orbell S., Hodgkins S., Sheeran P. Implementation intentions and the theory of planned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1997; 23 (9):945–954. doi: 10.1177/0146167297239004. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Orbell S., Sheeran P. Inclined abstainers': A problem for predicting health related behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology. 1998; 37 (2):151–165. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1998.tb01162.x. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Orbell S., Szczepura A., Weller D., Gumber A., Hagger M.S. South Asian ethnicity, socio-economic status and psychological mediators of faecal occult blood colorectal screening participation: A prospective test of a process model. Health Psychology. 2017; 36 (12):1161–1172. doi: 10.1037/hea0000525. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ouellette J.A., Wood W. Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 1998; 124 (1):54–74. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.54. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peterson L.M., Helweg-Larsen M., DiMuccio S. Descriptive norms and prototypes predict COVID-19 prevention cognitions and behaviors in the United States: Applying the prototype willingness model to pandemic mitigation. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2021; 55 (11):1089–1103. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaab075. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Phillips N. The coronavirus is here to stay — here's what that means. Nature. 2021; 590 :382–384. doi: 10.1038/d41586-021-00396-2. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Prestwich A., Sniehotta F.F., Whittington C., Dombrowski S.U., Rogers L., Michie S. Does theory influence the effectiveness of health behavior interventions? Meta-analysis. Health Psychology. 2014; 33 (5):465–474. doi: 10.1037/a0032853. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Prestwich A., Webb T.L., Conner M. Using theory to develop and test interventions to promote changes in health behaviour: Evidence, issues, and recommendations. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2015; 5 :1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.02.011. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Protogerou C., Johnson B.T., Hagger M.S. An integrated model of condom use in sub-saharan African youth: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology. 2018; 37 (6):586–602. doi: 10.1037/hea0000604. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Quaresma A.M., Palmeira A.L., Martins S.S., Minderico C.S., Sardinha L.B. Effect of a school-based intervention on physical activity and quality of life through serial mediation of social support and exercise motivation: The PESSOA program. Health Education Research. 2014; 29 (6):906–917. doi: 10.1093/her/cyu056. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rabin C., Dutra S. Predicting engagement in behaviors to reduce the spread of COVID-19: The roles of the health belief model and political party affiliation. Psychology Health & Medicine. 2021 doi: 10.1080/13548506.2021.1921229. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Raza A., Ali Q., Hussain T. Role of knowledge, behavior, norms, and e-guidelines in controlling the spread of COVID-19: Evidence from Pakistan. Environmental Science and Pollution Research. 2021; 28 (30):40329–40345. doi: 10.1007/s11356-020-10931-9. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rhodes R.E., Boudreau F., Weman Josefsson K., Ivarsson A. Mediators of physical activity behavior change interventions among adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review. 2020; 15 (2):272–286. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2019.1706614. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rhodes R.E., Courneya K.S., Hayduk L.A. Does personality moderate the theory of planned behavior in the exercise domain? Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology. 2002; 24 :120–132. doi: 10.1123/jsep.24.2.120. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rhodes R.E., Saelens B.E., Sauvage-Mar C. Understanding physical activity through interactions between the built environment and social cognition: A systematic review. Sports Medicine. 2018; 48 (8):1893–1912. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0934-0. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rivis A., Sheeran P., Armitage C.J. Expanding the affective and normative components of the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analysis of anticipated affect and moral norms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2009; 39 (12):2985–3019. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00558.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rogers R.W. A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change. Journal of Psychology. 1975; 91 (1):93–114. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1975.9915803. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rosenstock I.M. Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Education Monographs. 1974; 2 :328–335. doi: 10.1177/109019817400200403. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rothman A.J., Klein W.M.P., Sheeran P. In: The handbook of behavior change. Hagger M.S., Cameron L.D., Hamilton K., Hankonen N., Lintunen T., editors. Cambridge University Press; 2020. Moving from theoretical principles to intervention strategies: Applying the experimental medicine approach; pp. 285–299. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rothman A.J., Sheeran P. The operating conditions framework: Integrating mechanisms and moderators in health behavior interventions. Health Psychology. 2020 doi: 10.1037/hea0001026. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sallis J.F., Owen N., Fisher E.B. In: Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice. 5th ed. Glanz K., Rimer B.K., Viswanath K., editors. Jossey-Bass; 2015. Ecological models of health behavior; pp. 43–64. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Salmon J., Hesketh K.D., Arundell L., Downing K.L., Biddle S.J.H. In: The handbook of behavior change. Hagger M.S., Cameron L.D., Hamilton K., Hankonen N., Lintunen T., editors. Cambridge University Press; 2020. Changing behavior using ecological models; pp. 237–250. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schüz B., Brick C., Wilding S., Conner M.T. Socioeconomic status moderates the effects of health cognitions on health behaviors within participants: Two multibehavior studies. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2020; 54 (1):36–48. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaz023. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schüz B., Conner M., Wilding S., Alhawtan R., Prestwich A., Norman P. Do socio-structural factors moderate the effects of health cognitions on COVID-19 protection behaviours? Social Science & Medicine. 2021; 285 doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114261. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwarzer R. Modeling health behaviour change: How to predict and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors. Applied Psychology: International Review. 2008; 57 (1):1–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00325.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwarzer R., Hamilton K. In: Handbook of behavior change. Hagger M.S., Cameron L.D., Hamilton K., Hankonen N., Lintunen T., editors. Cambridge University Press; 2020. Changing behavior using the health action process approach; pp. 89–103. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sheeran P., Gollwitzer P.M., Bargh J.A. Nonconscious processes and health. Health Psychology. 2013; 32 (5):460–473. doi: 10.1037/a0029203. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sheeran P., Klein W.M.P., Rothman A.J. Health behavior change: Moving from observation to intervention. Annual Review of Psychology. 2017; 68 (1):573–600. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044007. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sheeran P., Maki A., Montanaro E., Avishai-Yitshak A., Bryan A., Klein W.M.P., et al. The impact of changing attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy on health-related intentions and behavior: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology. 2016; 35 (11):1178–1188. doi: 10.1037/hea0000387. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sheeran P., Webb T.L. The intention–behavior gap. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2016; 10 (9):503–518. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12265. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sheeran P., Wright C.E., Avishai A., Villegas M.E., Lindemans J.W., Klein W.M.P., et al. Self-determination theory interventions for health behavior change: Meta-analysis and meta-analytic structural equation modeling of randomized controlled trials. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2020; 88 (8):726–737. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000501. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sheeran P., Wright C.E., Avishai A., Villegas M.E., Rothman A.J., Klein W.M.P. Does increasing autonomous motivation or perceived competence lead to health behavior change? A meta-analysis. Health Psychology. 2021; 40 (10):706–716. doi: 10.1037/hea0001111. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sherman S.M., Smith L.E., Sim J., Amlôt R., Cutts M., Dasch H., et al. COVID-19 vaccination intention in the UK: Results from the COVID-19 vaccination acceptability study (CoVAccS), a nationally representative cross-sectional survey. Human Vaccines & Immunotherapeutics. 2021; 17 (6):1612–1621. doi: 10.1080/21645515.2020.1846397. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shiloh S., Peleg S., Nudelman G. Vaccination against COVID-19: A longitudinal trans-theoretical study to determine factors that predict intentions and behavior. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2021 doi: 10.1093/abm/kaab101. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith S.R., Hagger M.S., Keech J.J., Moyers S.A., Hamilton K. Improving hand hygiene behavior using a novel theory-based intervention during the COVID-19 pandemic. 2021. from. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Strack F., Deutsch R. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2004; 8 :220–247. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Suls J., Mogavero J.N., Falzon L., Pescatello L.S., Hennessy E.A., Davidson K.W. Health behaviour change in cardiovascular disease prevention and management: Meta-review of behavior change techniques to affect self-regulation. Health Psychology Review. 2020; 14 (1):43–65. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2019.1691622. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tong K.K., He M., Wu A.M.S., Dang L., Chen J.H. Cognitive factors influencing COVID-19 vaccination intentions: An application of the protection motivation theory using a probability community sample. Vaccines. 2021; 9 (10):1170. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trafimow D. The role of auxiliary assumptions for the validity of manipulations and measures. Theory & Psychology. 2012; 22 (4):486–498. doi: 10.1177/0959354311429996. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Usami Satoshi. On the differences between general cross-lagged panel model and random-intercept cross-lagged panel model: Interpretation of cross-lagged parameters and model choice. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2021; 28 (3):331–344. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2020.1821690. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vandrevala T., Montague A., Terry P., Fielder M.D. Willingness of the UK public to volunteer for testing in relation to the COVID-19 pandemic. BMC Public Health. 2022; 22 (1):565. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-12848-z. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Varol T., Crutzen R., Schneider F., Mesters I., Ruiter R.A.C., Kok G., et al. Selection of determinants of students' adherence to COVID-19 guidelines and translation into a brief intervention. Acta Psychologica. 2021; 219 doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103400. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Verplanken B. Beyond frequency: Habit as mental construct. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2006; 45 (3):639–656. doi: 10.1348/014466605X49122. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Verplanken B., Orbell S. Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2003; 33 (6):1313–1330. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Webb T.L., Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin. 2006; 132 (2):249–268. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weinstein N.D. Misleading tests of health behavior theories. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2007; 33 :1–10. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3301_1. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • West R., Michie S., Rubin G.J., Amlôt R. Applying principles of behaviour change to reduce SARS-CoV-2 transmission. Nature Human Behaviour. 2020; 4 (5):451–459. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0887-9. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • WHO . 2020. WHO Director-General's opening remarks at the media briefing on COVID-19 - 11 March 2020. https://www.who.int/director-general/speeches/detail/who-director-general-s-opening-remarks-at-the-media-briefing-on-covid-19---11-march-2020 Retrieved March 26, 2022, from. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wood W., Labrecque J.S., Lin P.-Y., Rünger D. In: Dual-process theories of the social mind. Sherman J.W., Gawronski B., Trope Y., editors. Guildford Press; 2014. Habits in dual process models; pp. 371–385. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wood W., Quinn J.M., Kashy D.A. Habits in everyday life: Thought, emotion, and action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002; 83 (6):1281–1297. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1281. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wu S., Neill R., De Foo C., Chua A.Q., Jung A.-S., Haldane V., et al. Aggressive containment, suppression, and mitigation of covid-19: Lessons learnt from eight countries. BMJ. 2021; 375 doi: 10.1136/bmj-2021-067508. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Xu P., Cheng J. Individual differences in social distancing and mask-wearing in the pandemic of COVID-19: The role of need for cognition, self-control and risk attitude. Personality and Individual Differences. 2021; 175 doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110706. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yahaghi R., Ahmadizade S., Fotuhi R., Taherkhani E., Ranjbaran M., Buchali Z., et al. Fear of COVID-19 and perceived COVID-19 infectability supplement theory of planned behavior to explain Iranians' intention to get COVID-19 vaccinated. Vaccines. 2021; 9 (7):684. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9070684. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yu Y., Lau J.T.F., Lau M.M.C. Levels and factors of social and physical distancing based on the Theory of Planned Behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic among Chinese adults. Translational Behavioral Medicine. 2021; 11 (5):1179–1186. doi: 10.1093/tbm/ibaa146. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zettler I., Schild C., Lilleholt L., Kroencke L., Utesch T., Moshagen M., et al. The role of personality in COVID-19-related perceptions, evaluations, and behaviors: Findings across five samples, nine traits, and 17 criteria. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2022; 13 (1):299–310. doi: 10.1177/19485506211001680. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang C.Q., Zhang R., Schwarzer R., Hagger M.S. A meta-analysis of the health action process approach. Health Psychology. 2019; 38 (7):623–637. doi: 10.1037/hea0000728. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Clinical Practice Guideline for the Treatment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)

Case Example: Jill, a 32-year-old Afghanistan War Veteran

This case example explains how Jill's therapist used a cognitive intervention with a written worksheet as a starting point for engaging in Socratic dialogue. 

About this Example

description

Jill's Story

Jill, a 32-year-old Afghanistan war veteran, had been experiencing PTSD symptoms for more than five years. She consistently avoided thoughts and images related to witnessing her fellow service members being hit by an improvised explosive device (IED) while driving a combat supply truck. Over the years, Jill became increasingly depressed and began using alcohol on a daily basis to help assuage her PTSD symptoms. She had difficulties in her employment, missing many days of work, and she reported feeling disconnected and numb around her husband and children. In addition to a range of other PTSD symptoms, Jill had a recurring nightmare of the event in which she was the leader of a convoy and her lead truck broke down. She waved the second truck forward, the truck that hit the IED, while she and her fellow service members on the first truck worked feverishly to repair it. Consistent with the traumatic event, her nightmare included images of her and the service members on the first truck smiling and waving at those on the second truck, and the service members on the second truck making fun of the broken truck and their efforts to fix it — “Look at that piece of junk truck — good luck getting that clunker fixed.”

After a thorough assessment of her PTSD and comorbid symptoms, psychoeducation about PTSD symptoms, and a rationale for using trauma focused cognitive interventions, Jill received 10 sessions of cognitive therapy for PTSD. She was first assigned cognitive worksheets to begin self-monitoring events, her thoughts about these events, and consequent feelings. These worksheets were used to sensitize Jill to the types of cognitions that she was having about current day events and to appraisals that she had about the explosion. For example, one of the thoughts she recorded related to the explosion was, “I should have had them wait and not had them go on.” She recorded her related feeling to be guilt. Jill’s therapist used this worksheet as a starting point for engaging in Socratic dialogue, as shown in the following example:

Therapist: Jill, do you mind if I ask you a few questions about this thought that you noticed, “I should have had them wait and not had them go on?”

Client: Sure.

Therapist: Can you tell me what the protocol tells you to do in a situation in which a truck breaks down during a convoy?

Client: You want to get the truck repaired as soon as possible, because the point of a convoy is to keep the trucks moving so that you aren’t sitting ducks.

Therapist: The truck that broke down was the lead truck that you were on. What is the protocol in that case?

Client: The protocol says to wave the other trucks through and keep them moving so that you don’t have multiple trucks just sitting there together more vulnerable.

Therapist: Okay. That’s helpful for me to understand. In light of the protocol you just described and the reasons for it, why do you think you should have had the second truck wait and not had them go on?

Client: If I hadn’t have waved them through and told them to carry on, this wouldn’t have happened. It is my fault that they died. (Begins to cry)

Therapist: (Pause) It is certainly sad that they died. (Pause) However, I want us to think through the idea that you should have had them wait and not had them go on, and consequently that it was your fault. (Pause) If you think back about what you knew at the time — not what you know now 5 years after the outcome — did you see anything that looked like a possible explosive device when you were scanning the road as the original lead truck?

Client: No. Prior to the truck breaking down, there was nothing that we noticed. It was an area of Iraq that could be dangerous, but there hadn’t been much insurgent activity in the days and weeks prior to it happening.

Therapist: Okay. So, prior to the explosion, you hadn’t seen anything suspicious.

Client: No.

Therapist: When the second truck took over as the lead truck, what was their responsibility and what was your responsibility at that point?

Client: The next truck that Mike and my other friends were on essentially became the lead truck, and I was responsible for trying to get my truck moving again so that we weren’t in danger.

Therapist: Okay. In that scenario then, would it be Mike and the others’ jobs to be scanning the environment ahead for potential dangers?

Client: Yes, but I should have been able to see and warn them.

Therapist: Before we determine that, how far ahead of you were Mike and the others when the explosion occurred?

Client: Oh (pause), probably 200 yards?

Therapist: 200 yards—that’s two football fields’ worth of distance, right?

Client: Right.

Therapist: You’ll have to educate me. Are there explosive devices that you wouldn’t be able to detect 200 yards ahead?

Client: Absolutely.

Therapist: How about explosive devices that you might not see 10 yards ahead?

Client: Sure. If they are really good, you wouldn’t see them at all.

Therapist: So, in light of the facts that you didn’t see anything at the time when you waved them through at 200 yards behind and that they obviously didn’t see anything 10 yards ahead before they hit the explosion, and that protocol would call for you preventing another danger of being sitting ducks, help me understand why you wouldn’t have waved them through at that time? Again, based on what you knew at the time?

Client: (Quietly) I hadn’t thought about the fact that Mike and the others obviously didn’t see the device at 10 yards, as you say, or they would have probably done something else. (Pause) Also, when you say that we were trying to prevent another danger at the time of being “sitting ducks,” it makes me feel better about waving them through.

Therapist: Can you describe the type of emotion you have when you say, “It makes me feel better?”

Client: I guess I feel less guilty.

Therapist: That makes sense to me. As we go back and more accurately see the reality of what was really going on at the time of this explosion, it is important to notice that it makes you feel better emotionally. (Pause) In fact, I was wondering if you had ever considered that, in this situation, you actually did exactly what you were supposed to do and that something worse could have happened had you chosen to make them wait?

Client: No. I haven’t thought about that.

Therapist: Obviously this was an area that insurgents were active in if they were planting explosives. Is it possible that it could have gone down worse had you chosen not to follow protocol and send them through?

Client: Hmmm. I hadn’t thought about that either.

Therapist: That’s okay. Many people don’t think through what could have happened if they had chosen an alternative course of action at the time or they assume that there would have only been positive outcomes if they had done something different. I call it “happily ever after” thinking — assuming that a different action would have resulted in a positive outcome. (Pause) When you think, “I did a good job following protocol in a stressful situation that may have prevented more harm from happening,” how does that make you feel?

Client: It definitely makes me feel less guilty.

Therapist: I’m wondering if there is any pride that you might feel?

Client: Hmmm...I don’t know if I can go that far.

Therapist: What do you mean?

Client: It seems wrong to feel pride when my friends died.

Therapist: Is it possible to feel both pride and sadness in this situation? (Pause) Do you think Mike would hold it against you for feeling pride, as well as sadness for his and others’ losses?

Client: Mike wouldn’t hold it against me. In fact, he’d probably reassure me that I did a good job.

Therapist: (Pause) That seems really important for you to remember. It may be helpful to remind yourself of what you have discovered today, because you have some habits in thinking about this event in a particular way. We are also going to be doing some practice assignments that will help to walk you through your thoughts about what happened during this event, help you to remember what you knew at the time, and remind you how different thoughts can result in different feelings about what happened.

Client: I actually feel a bit better after this conversation. 

Another thought that Jill described in relation to the traumatic event was, “I should have seen the explosion was going to happen to prevent my friends from dying.” Her related feelings were guilt and self-directed anger. The therapist used this thought to introduce the cognitive intervention of "challenging thoughts" and provided a worksheet for practice. The therapist first provided education about the different types of thinking errors, including habitual thinking, all-or-none thinking, taking things out of context, overestimating probabilities, and emotional reasoning, as well as discussing other important factors, such as gathering evidence for and against the thought, evaluating the source of the information, and focusing on irrelevant factors. 

More specifically, Jill noted that she experienced 100 percent intensity of guilt and 75 percent intensity of anger at herself in relation to the thought "I should have seen the explosive device to prevent my friends from dying." She posed several challenging questions, including the notion that improvised explosive devices are meant to be concealed, that she is the source of the information (because others don't blame her), and that her feelings are not based on facts (i.e., she feels guilt and therefore must be guilty). She came up with the alternative thought, "The best explosive devices aren't seen and Mike (driver of the second truck) was a good soldier. If he saw something he would stopped or tried to evade it," which she rated as 90 percent confidence in believing. She consequently believed her original thought 10 percent, and re-rated her emotions as only 10 percent guilt and 5 percent anger at self.

Treating PTSD with cognitive-behavioral therapies: Interventions that work

This case example is reprinted with permission from: Monson, C. M. & Shnaider, P. (2014). Treating PTSD with cognitive-behavioral therapies: Interventions that work . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 

Other Case Examples

  • Cognitive Therapy Philip, a 60-year-old who was in a traffic accident (PDF, 294KB)
  • Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing Mike, a 32-year-old Iraq War Veteran
  • Narrative Exposure Therapy Eric, a 24-year-old Rwandan refugee living in Uganda (PDF, 28KB)
  • Prolonged Exposure Terry, a 42-year-old earthquake survivor

IMAGES

  1. Social Cognitive Theory: 10 Examples and Overview (2024)

    case study social cognitive theory

  2. Social Cognitive Theory PowerPoint Presentation Slides

    case study social cognitive theory

  3. PPT

    case study social cognitive theory

  4. Social Cognitive Theory Conceptual Framework

    case study social cognitive theory

  5. Bandura's Model of Social Cognitive Theory Stock Photo

    case study social cognitive theory

  6. Social cognitive theory of personality

    case study social cognitive theory

VIDEO

  1. Social Cognitive Theory Discussion

  2. Schoolwork Studies: Social Cognitive Theory

  3. LM Social Cognitive Theory 2

  4. Albert Bandura Theory

  5. IBPSYCH: SOCIOCULTURAL APPROACH: THE INDIVIDUAL ANDTHE GROUP: SOCIALCOGNITIVE THEORY: BANDURA PART 2

  6. THEORIES OF SOCIAL CHANGE

COMMENTS

  1. (PDF) Case study: Using social cognitive theory and social support

    The program was based on Ajzen's theory of planned behavior (2005), Bandura's social-cognitive theory (1997) and cognitive-behavior theory of behavioral change (Meichenbaum, 1993).

  2. A Systematic Review Exploring the Social Cognitive Theory of Self

    The social cognitive theory of self-regulation proposes that three main components of the theory, self-monitoring, self-judgement, and self-evaluation, contribute to self-regulation, and influence successful behaviour change. ... case studies, theses, reviews, summaries, commentaries, editorials, letters to the editor, or study protocols ...

  3. Theory and Application in Personality Science: The Case of Social

    Three central themes in social-cognitive conceptions are identified: (a) a focus on individuals rather than on summaries of individual differences in the population, (b) attention to causal mechanisms underlying action and experience and (c) the study of persons in context, including attention to psychological processes through which people ...

  4. Social Cognitive Personality Assessment: A Case Conceptualization

    Social-cognitive theory has had minimal influence on personality assessment and case conceptualization practices despite significant recent developments in theoretically driven assessment principles and strategies ... In our case study, S.L. benefitted from realizing that both her self-as-inferior/ruined schemata, as well as her inhibited ...

  5. Social cognitive theory-based health promotion in primary care practice

    All the 39 included studies in this scoping review integrated constructs of Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) in developing and delivering the intervention in the primary health care setting. " Self-efficacy: the degree of confidence an individual has in his/her ability to perform a behavior despite barriers " was the most utilized construct of ...

  6. Using Social Cognitive Theory to Predict Medication Compliance Behavior

    The social cognitive theory (SCT) is a theory of human behavior based on the assumption that expectations, thoughts, and beliefs influence's ones behavior and is shaped by the individuals' social environment. 18,19 This approach has not been tested for compliance behavior in depression patients. In this study, the SCT will be used to ...

  7. Theory and Application in Personality Science: The Case of Social

    Theory and Application in Personality Science: The Case of Social-cognitive Theory. August 2023. Psychology and Developing Societies 35 (2) DOI: 10.1177/09713336231178366. Authors: Daniel Cervone ...

  8. Case study: using social cognitive theory and social support coping

    In this case study, social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986, 2004) and social support as a way of coping (Vitaliano et al., 1985) have been selected to overcome barriers created by low levels of self-confidence and perceived lack of support in the context of breastfeeding. Thus, the application of the two theories in this case study is designed ...

  9. Social cognitive theory

    Social cognitive theory (SCT), used in psychology, ... In this case, the children in this experiment reenacted the model of violence they directly learned from the video. ... Many social cognitive studies have explored the impact of violence in the media on human behavior. In SCT's infancy, violence is what Bandura focused his study around as ...

  10. (PDF) Clinical Application of Social Cognitive Theory: A Novel

    Clinical Application of Social Cognitive Theory: A Novel Personality Assessment Procedure and a Case Study of Personality Coherence July 2021 European Journal of Personality 36(2):089020702110283

  11. Social Cognitive Theory

    Abstract. Social cognitive theory is a contemporary theory of learning that emphasizes learning from the social environment. The conceptual model of Bandura's social cognitive theory reflects reciprocal interactions between personal, behavioral, and social/environmental variables. The theory stresses the key roles played by vicarious, symbolic ...

  12. Clinical Application of Social Cognitive Theory: A Novel Personality

    We present a novel personality assessment method that applies social cognitive personality theory, and more specifically, the Knowledge and Appraisal Personality Architecture model (KAPA; Cervone, 2004; 2021; see Scott & Cervone, 2016).Our assessment method generates descriptions of how personality structures, including temperament, beliefs, goals, and evaluative standards, are activated in ...

  13. Motivation and social cognitive theory

    1. Introduction. Social cognitive theory is a psychological perspective on human functioning that emphasizes the critical role played by the social environment on motivation, learning, and self-regulation (Schunk & Usher, 2019).Because there are different social cognitive theoretical perspectives, to focus this article the discussion is limited to the social cognitive theory proposed by ...

  14. Social Cognitive Theory: Definition and Examples

    Updated on July 15, 2024. Social cognitive theory is a learning theory developed by the renowned Stanford psychology professor Albert Bandura. The theory provides a framework for understanding how people actively shape and are shaped by their environment. In particular, the theory details the processes of observational learning and modeling ...

  15. The social‐cognitive clinician: On the implications of social cognitive

    We review Bandura's contributions to cognitive-behavioural theory, research and practice. His basic research on the causal role of cognitive processes in social learning was a major factor in the emergence of cognitive-behavioural therapies in the 1970s.His investigations on observational learning and self-efficacy beliefs led to the development of guided mastery therapy, a specific cognitive ...

  16. Albert Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory

    Social cognitive theory emphasizes the learning that occurs within a social context. In this view, people are active agents who can both influence and are influenced by their environment. The theory was founded most prominently by Albert Bandura, who is also known for his work on observational learning, self-efficacy, and reciprocal determinism.

  17. (PDF) Social Cognitive Theory

    Abstract. In conceptualizations presented in social cognitive theory (SCT), humans are not passive objects shaped and shepherded by contingent consequences of an environment. People are agentic ...

  18. Social cognition theories and behavior change in COVID-19: A conceptual

    These studies have progressed knowledge on the social cognition correlates of COVID-19 preventive behaviors, and we look to future research that further elicits specific beliefs concerning COVID-19 prevention and examines their effects on preventive behaviors. 2.3.3. Study design and inferences.

  19. Social cognitive theory

    Social cognitive theory - IB Psych Matters

  20. Social cognitive theory and physical activity ...

    Albert Bandura's (1986, 2001) social cognitive theory (SCT) represents one of the most studied theories in the field of psychology, with applications in contexts as diverse as education, healthcare, rehabilitation, the legal system, business, and indeed sport and exercise. It is positioned as a theory of human behavior with integrative principles of broad applicability (Bandura, 1998).

  21. Social Cognitive Theory and Health Care: Analysis and Evaluation

    Abstract. Social Cognitiv e Theory explains how different personal, environm ental and cognitiv e factors influenc e human behavior. and it has been an important source of knowle dge in the social ...

  22. Case Study: Cognitive Behavioral Therapy

    Monson, C. M. & Shnaider, P. (2014). Treating PTSD with cognitive-behavioral therapies: Interventions that work. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Updated July 31, 2017. Date created: 2017. This case example explains how Jill's therapist used a cognitive intervention with a written worksheet as a starting point for engaging in ...